Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Comment on “Human-like hand use in Australopithecus africanus”

Skinner and colleagues (Research Article, 23 January 2015, p. 395), based on metacarpal trabecular bone structure, argue that Australopithecus africanus employed human-like dexterity for stone tool making and use 3 million years ago. However, their evolutionary and biological assumptions are misinformed, failing to refute the previously existing hypothesis that human-like manipulation preceded systematized stone tool manufacture, as indicated by the fossil record.

T E C H N I C A L CO M M E N T ◥ HUMAN EVOLUTION Comment on “Human-like hand use in Australopithecus africanus” Sergio Almécija,1,2,3* Ian J. Wallace,4 Stefan Judex,5 David M. Alba,3 Salvador Moyà-Solà6 Skinner and colleagues (Research Article, 23 January 2015, p. 395), based on metacarpal trabecular bone structure, argue that Australopithecus africanus employed human-like dexterity for stone tool making and use 3 million years ago. However, their evolutionary and biological assumptions are misinformed, failing to refute the previously existing hypothesis that human-like manipulation preceded systematized stone tool manufacture, as indicated by the fossil record. S kinner et al. (1) analyze metacarpal trabecular bone structure in the 3-million-yearold hominin Australopithecus africanus and infer based on these data that this taxon “was capable of habitual and forceful humanlike opposition of the thumb and fingers during [...] tool-related behaviors, providing morphological evidence of committed tool use in a hominin hitherto considered not to be capable of these behaviors” (1). Specifically, they found that the trabecular organization of the pollical metacarpal of A. africanus is, in some respects, more humanlike than chimpanzee-like, which they interpret as “morphological evidence…that can be linked to behavior and hand use during life” (1), with clear allusions to stone tool making and use throughout the article. Although we appreciate their effort to investigate a novel aspect of fossil morphology like trabecular structure, we note here limitations in the proposed evolutionary importance of their findings, which together with misinformed assumptions about bone mechanobiology make many of their conclusions unwarranted. We hope that the concerns that we raise will help promote a constructive discussion dealing with the complex topic of the relationships between form and function and redirect future research studies in human evolution. It is well known that, like humans, all living great apes make and use tools, and some chimpanzees and capuchin monkeys even engage in 1 Department of Anatomical Sciences, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY 11794, USA. 2Center for the Advanced Study of Human Paleobiology, Department of Anthropology, The George Washington University, Science and Engineering Hall, 800 22nd Street NW, Washington, DC 20052, USA. 3 Institut Català de Paleontologia Miquel Crusafont, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Edifici ICTA-ICP, Carrer de les Columnes s/n, Campus de la UAB, 08193 Cerdanyola del Vallès, Barcelona, Spain. 4Department of Anthropology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY 11794, USA. 5 Department of Biomedical Engineering, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY 11794, USA. 6ICREA at Institut Català de Paleontologia Miquel Crusafont and Unitat d’Antropologia Biològica (Departament BABVE), Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Edifici ICTA-CP, Carrer de les Columnes s/n, Campus de la UAB, 08193 Cerdanyola del Vallès, Barcelona, Spain. *Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected] SCIENCE sciencemag.org regular stone tool use (2). Humans, however, display especially advanced manual dexterity facilitated by a unique (among modern hominoids) type of interaction between the proximal pulps of our thumb and fingers (3). The primary anatomical requirement for this pad-to-pad precision grasping is having a long thumb relative to finger lengths (3–5). Previous analyses of hand bone morphology in australopiths (4, 6–8) and the Miocene hominin Orrorin (5) have provided compelling evidence for pad-to-pad precision grasping before the widespread occurrence of flaked stone tools, which indicates that a basic human-like hand structure is plesiomorphic for modern humans (Fig. 1). The emerging evolutionary picture is that increased reliance on bipedalism and enhanced manipulation were very early apomorphies of the hominin lineage, as a part of our original adaptive shift from apes in relation to new foraging strategies (5, 7). Thus, the eventual application of human-like hand structure to stone tool flaking and use was almost certainly an exaptation, not an adaptation (4). In this light, the inference by Skinner et al. of human-like hand use among australopiths is neither unprecedented nor unexpected. Skinner et al. downplay previous evidence for human-like hand use among australopiths by arguing that because earlier studies were focused exclusively on external (cortical) bone features, they do not necessarily provide insight into how hominins were actually using their hands during life. As they see it, “external morphology can be ambiguous, as some features can be retentions from the ancestral condition and may not be functionally important” (1). The structure of internal trabecular bone, they argue, is a truer reflection of in vivo loading than cortical structure and less influenced by nonmechanical factors such as phylogeny. This claim is based on a simplified account of bone functional adaptation that trabecular remodeling throughout life is optimized to add and preserve bone where loading occurs and resorb bone where it does not. However, it is important to recognize that trabecular structure alone has little bearing on individual capacity to achieve human-like hand grips. In fact, no type of trabecular organization will enable an individual to attain human-like grips if the external morphology of their hand bones does not permit it. Therefore, Skinner and colleagues’ conclusions rely on the circular reasoning that the external morphology of australopith hands actually allowed human-like grips, which weakens the basic premise of their study that trabecular structure is a more informative signal than external features. Furthermore, Skinner et al.’s interpretation of trabecular bone functional adaptation is problematic for at least three reasons. First, while trabecular bone clearly can respond to mechanical signals, loads must be dynamic to be osteogenic (9). Relatively static loads associated with tool grasping [like those shown for the pollical metacarpal in figure 1 in (1)] have not been associated with enhanced trabecular morphology, independent of the magnitude of the applied force. Second, there is little evidence that trabecular bone is less influenced by genetics, or more influenced by loading, than cortical bone. Heritability estimates for trabecular density in humans range between 59 and 73%, whereas estimates for cortical density are between 17 and 42% [see references in (10)]. Moreover, human studies have shown that cortical morphology of weight-bearing elements scales far better with body mass (a proxy for mechanical loading) than trabecular architecture [e.g., (11)]. Third, if trabecular organization indeed correlates strongly with predicted loading conditions, then this should be evident throughout the skeleton, especially in weightbearing elements. However, numerous previous attempts to identify functional loading signals in the trabecular structure of weight-bearing elements in humans and other primates have most often been unsuccessful (12). Thus, why mechanical signals induced by object manipulation should be uniquely discernible in hominin pollical metacarpals is unclear. Skinner et al. base their conclusions on their previous comparative analysis of trabecular structure in the hominoid third metacarpal (13). However, this analysis was unable to separate humans from gibbons or orangutans [figure 4 in (13)], and it failed to provide any direct evidence that observed differences between taxa were actually due to variation in lifetime mechanical loading. Inherently, extrapolating from this potentially phylogenetically dependent correlation in the third metacarpal to all metacarpals renders the conclusions of Skinner et al. even more speculative. Nevertheless, even if one assumes that metacarpal trabecular structure closely reflects lifetime loading, Skinner et al.’s comparative evidence linking form to function is limited. Although the authors found that “[d]ifferences in the distribution of trabecular bone across apes and humans were also found in Mc3 and Mc5” (1), their pairwise comparisons [table S3 in (1)] indicate that this is only true for the fifth metacarpal head, for which no australopiths were sampled [table 1 in (1)]. For the third metacarpal head, differences in trabecular organization were only found between humans and African apes [table S3 in 5 JUNE 2015 • VOL 348 ISSUE 6239 1101-a Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on June 4, 2015 R ES E A RC H R ES E A RC H | T E C H N I C A L CO M M E N T ca. 55.0 Euprimates: prehensile hands and flattened nails facilitate grasping Plesiomorphic apes: generalized hand proportions (moderate thumb-fingers ratio) and highly mobile sellar trapezio-metacarpal joint enhances thumb opposition ca. 18.0 ca. 16.0 ca. 7.0 6.0 Great apes: habitual tool use and tool making involving organic materials (e.g., leaves, sticks) related to increased encephalization Last common ancestor of Pan and Homo: tool use involving unintentionally modified stones (probably similiar to Taï Forest chimpanzees) Orrorin tugenensis: thumb and femoral morphology indicate human-like pad-to-pad precision grasping plus habitual bipedalism, (implying new foraging strategies in early hominins) 5.0 4.0 3.0 2.0 1.0 0.0 Ma Australopiths: human-like intrinsic hand proportions and pad-to-pad precision grasping (plesiomorphic for australopiths) possible sporadic stone tool making and butchering Widespread systematized stone tool production: commonly attributed to early Homo species (some fossils show slightly enlarged braincases) More elaborate stone tools: associated with Homo erectus and later hominins, which exhibited both an expanded endocranial volume and overall modern human-like body form Homo sapiens: highly encephalized and well-developed culture, very advanced manipulative skills and technology Fig. 1. Simplified timeline showing major evolutionary advents in human manipulative capabilities. The fossil record indicates that the story of human advanced manipulation has very early origins, predating the widespread appearance of systematized flaked stone tool manufacture. Basic human-like hand proportions allowing for refined manipulation have been inferred in Orrorin and all australopith species and confirmed by the virtually complete hand of Australopithecus sediba. The morphological results of Skinner et al. merely support these previous findings. Ma, million years ago. (1)], with A. africanus showing values closer to apes than humans. Ultimately, Skinner et al.’s numerical results support human-like hand use in A. africanus based only on certain similarities with the trabecular pattern of the pollical metacarpal base of humans as compared to chimpanzees. In our opinion, it is overly risky to draw profound conclusions about hominin paleobiol- 1101-a 5 JUNE 2015 • VOL 348 ISSUE 6239 ogy based solely on a comparative sample of two taxa (Pan versus Homo). It is noteworthy that a previous inference (14) of australopith stone tool manufacture based on pollical metacarpal external morphology that relied on a comparative sample of only chimpanzees and humans was no longer supported once gorillas were included in the analysis [i.e., gorillas were similar to hominins (15)]. A more credible test relating pollical metacarpal trabecular structure to manipulative behaviors would incorporate not only gorillas (as they did with rays three and five) but also other primates with dexterous hands like Theropithecus (specialized forager) or the Taï Forest chimpanzees and Cebus (stone tool users). Such a strategy would permit evaluation of whether the observed morphological signatures of Skinner et al. are ecophenotypic (as they argue) instead of merely representing a hominin phylogenetic signal. Ultimately, the results of Skinner et al., far from demonstrating strong evidence of stone tool manufacture and use among australopiths, support previous observations reached on the basis of external morphology that australopith hands were, in some ways, more similar to those of humans than to apes, and thus were likely capable of human-like manipulation (4–8). However, this does not mean that human hands evolved for the sole purpose of making and using stone tools, for which cognitive capabilities should be also considered, as Napier and others indicated before (3). Note added in proof: The recently described lithic artifacts from Lomekwi 3, northwestern Kenya, are claimed to represent the earliest evidence of intentional stone tool production at 3.3 Ma [S. Harmand et al. (16)]. This discovery is consistent with human-like manual dexterity being an ancient adaptation among hominins. Even so, as Harmand et al. argue, and in agreement with our view, the decisive adaptation enabling “Lomekwian” stone knapping (to yetunidentified hominins) was likely neurological. REFERENCES AND NOTES 1. M. M. Skinner et al., Science 347, 395–399 (2015). 2. M. A. Panger, A. S. Brooks, B. G. Richmond, B. Wood, Evol. Anthropol. 11, 235–245 (2002). 3. J. Napier, Nature 196, 409–411 (1962). 4. D. M. Alba, S. Moyà-Solà, M. Köhler, J. Hum. Evol. 44, 225–254 (2003). 5. S. Almécija, S. Moyà-Solà, D. M. Alba, PLOS ONE 5, e11727 (2010). 6. D. J. Green, A. D. Gordon, J. Hum. Evol. 54, 705–719 (2008). 7. S. Almécija, D. M. Alba, J. Hum. Evol. 73, 88–92 (2014). 8. T. L. Kivell, J. M. Kibii, S. E. Churchill, P. Schmid, L. R. Berger, Science 333, 1411–1417 (2011). 9. T. Sugiyama, J. S. Price, L. E. Lanyon, Bone 46, 314–321 (2010). 10. L. M. Yerges et al., J. Bone Miner. Res. 25, 330–338 (2010). 11. J. D. Schipilow, H. M. Macdonald, A. M. Liphardt, M. Kan, S. K. Boyd, Bone 56, 281–289 (2013). 12. C. N. Shaw, T. M. Ryan, Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 147, 187–200 (2012). 13. Z. J. Tsegai et al., PLOS ONE 8, e78781 (2013). 14. R. L. Susman, Science 265, 1570–1573 (1994). 15. W. C. McGrew et al., Science 268, 586–589 (1995). 16. S. Harmand et al., Nature 521, 310–315 (2015). AC KNOWLED GME NTS We thank M. Skinner, T. Kivell, and their coauthors for their willingness to contribute to a collegial discussion on this interesting (although complex) topic. 8 February 2015; accepted 19 March 2015 10.1126/science.aaa8414 sciencemag.org SCIENCE Comment on ''Human-like hand use in Australopithecus africanus'' Sergio Almécija et al. Science 348, 1101 (2015); DOI: 10.1126/science.aaa8414 This copy is for your personal, non-commercial use only. If you wish to distribute this article to others, you can order high-quality copies for your colleagues, clients, or customers by clicking here. Permission to republish or repurpose articles or portions of articles can be obtained by following the guidelines here. Updated information and services, including high-resolution figures, can be found in the online version of this article at: http://www.sciencemag.org/content/348/6239/1101.1.full.html A list of selected additional articles on the Science Web sites related to this article can be found at: http://www.sciencemag.org/content/348/6239/1101.1.full.html#related This article cites 16 articles, 4 of which can be accessed free: http://www.sciencemag.org/content/348/6239/1101.1.full.html#ref-list-1 This article has been cited by 1 articles hosted by HighWire Press; see: http://www.sciencemag.org/content/348/6239/1101.1.full.html#related-urls This article appears in the following subject collections: Anthropology http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/collection/anthro Science (print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published weekly, except the last week in December, by the American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. Copyright 2015 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title Science is a registered trademark of AAAS. Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on June 4, 2015 The following resources related to this article are available online at www.sciencemag.org (this information is current as of June 4, 2015 ):