3070 • The Journal of Neuroscience, April 17, 2019 • 39(16):3070 –3080
Systems/Circuits
Two Compasses in the Central Complex of the Locust Brain
Uta Pegel,1 X Keram Pfeiffer,2 Frederick Zittrell,1 Christine Scholtyssek,3 and XUwe Homberg1
Animal Physiology, Department of Biology and Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior, Philipps-Universität Marburg, 35032 Marburg, Germany, 2Behavioral
Physiology and Sociobiology (Zoology II), Biozentrum, University of Würzburg, Am Hubland, 97074 Würzburg, Germany, and 3School of Experimental
Psychology, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TU, United Kingdom
1
Many migratory insects rely on a celestial compass for spatial orientation. Several features of the daytime sky, all generated by the sun, can
be exploited for navigation. Two of these are the position of the sun and the pattern of polarized skylight. Neurons of the central complex
(CX), a group of neuropils in the central brain of insects, have been shown to encode sky compass cues. In desert locusts, the CX holds a
topographic, compass-like representation of the plane of polarized light (E-vector) presented from dorsal direction. In addition, these
neurons also encode the azimuth of an unpolarized light spot, likely representing the sun. Here, we investigate whether, in addition to
E-vector orientation, the solar azimuth is represented topographically in the CX. We recorded intracellularly from eight types of CX
neuron while stimulating animals of either sex with polarized blue light from zenithal direction and an unpolarized green light spot
rotating around the animal’s head at different elevations. CX neurons did not code for elevation of the unpolarized light spot. However,
two types of columnar neuron showed a linear correlation between innervated slice in the CX and azimuth tuning to the unpolarized green
light spot, consistent with an internal compass representation of solar azimuth. Columnar outputs of the CX also showed a topographic
representation of zenithal E-vector orientation, but the two compasses were not linked to each other. Combined stimulation with
unpolarized green and polarized blue light suggested that the two compasses interact in a nonlinear way.
Key words: central complex; head direction; insect brain; navigation; polarization vision; sky compass
Significance Statement
In the brain of the desert locust, neurons sensitive to the plane of celestial polarization are arranged like a compass in the slices of
the central complex (CX). These neurons, in addition, code for the horizontal direction of an unpolarized light cue possibly
representing the sun. We show here that horizontal directions are, in addition to E-vector orientations from the dorsal direction,
represented in a compass-like manner across the slices of the CX. However, the two compasses are not linked to each other, but
rather seem to interact in a cell-specific, nonlinear way. Our study confirms the role of the CX in signaling heading directions and
shows that different cues are used for this task.
Introduction
Many animals rely on visual cues for navigation. Some of them,
including certain insects, exploit global compass cues of the sky to
extract heading information and maintain directions during
walking and flight (Wehner, 1984; Merlin et al., 2012; Homberg,
2015). Sky compass cues are highly reliable due to their persistent
presence during locomotion (Gould, 1998; Frost and Mouritsen,
Received April 13, 2018; revised Jan. 10, 2019; accepted Jan. 29, 2019.
Author contributions: U.P., K.P., and U.H. designed research; U.P. performed research; U.P., F.Z., and C.S. contributed unpublished reagents/analytic tools; U.P. analyzed data; U.P. wrote the first draft of the paper; U.P., K.P.,
F.Z., C.S., and U.H. edited the paper; U.P. and U.H. wrote the paper.
This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (Grants HO 950/23-1 and HO 950/24-1). We
thank Erich Buchner and Christian Wegener (University of Würzburg) for supplying anti-synapsin antibodies and
Martina Kern for maintaining locust cultures.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Correspondence should be addressed to Uwe Homberg at
[email protected].
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0940-18.2019
Copyright © 2019 the authors
2006). In addition to direct sunlight, the polarization pattern and
the chromatic gradient across the sky, both generated by scattering of sunlight in the atmosphere, provide reference to the position of the sun. In addition to sky compass cues, insects also rely
on landmarks and perhaps even map-like mechanisms of orientation, especially in familiar terrain (Collett, 1992; Menzel et al.,
2005; Zars, 2009; Wystrach and Graham, 2012). Large landscape
features such as coastlines or mountain ranges may also serve as
guiding cues for long-distance migrators (Reppert et al., 2016).
Several insect species show orientation behavior dependent
on sky compass cues or, under laboratory settings, signals that
mimic zenithal sky polarization or solar position. These include
honey bees (von Frisch, 1949; Brines and Gould, 1979), desert
ants (Wehner and Müller, 2006), dung beetles (Dacke et al., 2003;
el Jundi et al., 2014b), fruit flies (Weir and Dickinson, 2012), field
crickets (Brunner and Labhart, 1987), and locusts (Mappes and
Homberg, 2004). All of these species possess specialized photoreceptors working as E-vector analyzers located in the dorsal rim
Pegel et al. • Two Compasses in the Central Complex
J. Neurosci., April 17, 2019 • 39(16):3070 –3080 • 3071
tation of zenithal polarized light as well as
for the azimuth of an unpolarized green
light spot, likely representing the sun
(Heinze and Reppert, 2011; el Jundi et al.,
2014a, 2015; Pegel et al., 2018). This raises
the question of whether the azimuth of the
sun, like the E-vector angle, is represented
topographically in the slices of the CX. If
so, an internal azimuth compass phase
shifted by 90° to the E-vector compass
would be expected because this is the angular distance between the zenithal
E-vector and the solar azimuth in the sky.
We show here that several types of columnar neuron in the locust CX represent, not
only zenithal E-vectors, but also azimuth
angles of unpolarized light cues in a
compass-like manner. However, the two
compasses are not simply connected by a
90° relationship.
Materials and Methods
Animals and preparation. Desert locusts (Schistocerca gregaria) were reared under crowded
conditions in a 12/12 h light/dark cycle. Only
sexually mature male and female animals at
Figure 1. Neuronal cell types and visual stimulation. A–C, Schematic illustration of tangential (red) and columnar (blue) least 1 week after final molt were used for exneurons of the sky compass network in the locust CX. Vertical lines mark the edges of slices occupied by arborizations of columnar periments. Animals were mounted onto a
neurons. Slices are termed R1–R8 (right hemisphere) and L1–L8 (left hemisphere). Fine processes illustrate likely dendritic input metal holder using dental wax with their anteregions of the neuron; dots represent varicose arborizations und thus likely presynaptic output regions. The filled lateral and medial rior–posterior body axis oriented vertically
bulb (LBU, MBU) in A indicate input synapses arranged in microglomerular complexes. LAL, Lateral accessory lobe; POTU, posterior (Pfeiffer et al., 2005). Wings and legs were cut
optic tubercle. D, Schematic illustration of visual stimulation. The light of a blue LED positioned in the zenith was passed through off. The head capsule was opened from antea rotating polarizer. A green LED appeared to the animal as an unpolarized light spot. It rotated around the animal’s head (green rior and fat tissue, tracheal air sacs, and gut
horizontal arrow) at elevations ranging from 20° to 60° (green vertical arrow).
were removed to reduce body movements.
Mouthparts, leg stumps, and abdomen were
immobilized by wax. Muscles close to the brain
areas of their compound eyes (Labhart and Meyer, 1999; Schmelwere cut for further stabilization. A small twisted metal wire was used to
ing et al., 2014).
support the brain from posterior. The neural sheath covering the central
Visual pathways from the dorsal rim areas of both compound
brain was removed to allow access for the electrode. During preparation
eyes converge on the central complex (CX), a group of midlineand intracellular recording the brain was immersed in locust saline (Clespanning neuropils in the brain (Homberg et al., 2011; Heinze,
ments and May, 1974) containing 0.09 mol l ⫺1 saccharose.
2014). The CX consists of the upper and lower division of the
Electrophysiology and visual stimulation. Sharp glass microelectrodes
central body (CBU and CBL, respectively), the paired noduli
were drawn from borosilicate capillaries (Hilgenberg) using a Flaming/
Brown horizontal puller (P-97; Sutter Instruments). Electrode tips were
(NO), and the protocerebral bridge (PB; Fig. 1A). In the desert
filled with 4% Neurobiotin (Vector Laboratories) diluted in 1 mol l ⫺1
locust, the CBU, CBL, and PB are structured in rows of 16 vertical
KCl. Electrode shanks were filled with 1 mol l ⫺1 KCl. Neuronal signals
slices. Many CX neurons arborize in adjacent bilateral structures,
were amplified 10⫻ by a custom-built amplifier (University of Regensthe lateral accessory lobes, and the medial and lateral bulbs. Sevburg),
visualized by an oscilloscope (DS 1052Eh; Rigol Technologies),
eral types of CX neuron are involved in the processing of polardigitized by an analog-to-digital converter (CED1401 plus; Cambridge
ized light. These include tangential neurons of the CBL (TL2)
Electronic Design) at a rate of 20 kHz, and stored on a PC using Spike2
serving as input neurons to the CX and contacting columnar
version 6.02 software (Cambridge Electronic Design). Neuronal reneurons of the CBL (CL1; Fig. 1A). CL1 neurons arborize in
sponses to polarized and unpolarized light stimuli were studied (Fig. 1D).
distinct slices of the PB, where they might contact tangential neuPolarized light was generated by passing light of a blue LED (Oslon SSL
rons (TB1; Fig. 1B). Columnar neurons, termed CPU1, CPU2,
80, LDCQ7P, 452 nm; Osram Opto Semiconductors, or LXML-PR01CP1, and CP2, likely serve as output neurons of the network (Fig.
0500, 447.5 nm, Philips Lumileds) through a polarizer (HNP’B; Polaroid). Both were positioned in the animal’s zenith (with respect to its
1 B, C).
natural head orientation) to stimulate the dorsal part of the eye. The
Functional studies provide strong evidence for a role of the CX
polarized light stimulus covered a visual angle of 32.5° or 18.6° and had
in navigational tasks (Ofstad et al., 2011; Varga et al., 2017). In
an intensity of 1.7 ⫻ 10 13 photons cm ⫺2 s ⫺1. The polarizer was rotated
locusts and fruit flies, the CX holds an internal representation of
at angular velocities of 40°/s or 36°/s. The unpolarized light spot was
head orientation relative to a visual reference. Whereas in fruit
generated by light from a green LED (LED535-series, 535 nm, Roithner
flies head orientation relative to bright landmarks is represented
Lasertechnik, or Oslon SSL 80, LT CP7P, 528 nm; Osram Opto Semiconin the ellipsoid body (corresponding to the CBL in other species;
ductors) passing through a diffusor. The unpolarized light stimulus apSeelig and Jayaraman, 2013) and the PB (Green et al., 2017), a
peared at a visual angle of 16.3° and had an intensity of 10 14 photons
topographic representation of zenithal E-vectors is present in the
cm ⫺2 s ⫺1. It was moved around the animal’s head at an elevation of 45°
PB of the locust (Heinze and Homberg, 2007). In dung beetles,
and an angular velocity of 40°/s or 36°/s. In experiments testing for
monarch butterflies, and locusts, CX neurons code for the orienelevation-dependent coding the elevation of the light spot could be
3072 • J. Neurosci., April 17, 2019 • 39(16):3070 –3080
changed to 20°, 30°, 40°, 50°, and 60° (Fig. 1D). At the end of the recording, Neurobiotin was injected into the neuron by applying a positive
constant current of ⬃1 nA for 1– 4 min.
Histology and image processing. Brains were dissected in locust saline,
immersed overnight at 4°C in fixative containing 4% PFA, 0.25% glutaraldehyde, and 0.2% saturated picric acid diluted in 0.1 mol l ⫺1 PBS.
Brains were stored for up to 2 weeks at 4°C in sodium phosphate buffer.
Subsequently, they were incubated in PBS with 0.3% Triton X-100 and
Cy3-conjugated streptavidin (1:1000) for 3 d, dehydrated in an ascending ethanol series (30%, 50%, 70%, 90%, 95%, 100%) with 15 min steps,
and cleared in a 1:1 mixture of 100% ethanol and methyl salicylate for 20
min, followed by 1 h in 100% methyl salicylate. Finally, brains were
embedded in Permount (Fisher Scientific) between two coverslips. For
synapsin immunostaining, brains were rehydrated in a decreasing ethanol series (100%, 95%, 90%, 70%, 50%, 30%) in 15 min steps, embedded
in albumin/gelatin, fixed overnight in 8% formaldehyde at 4°C, and sectioned in 130 m slices using a vibrating-blade microtome (VT 1000S;
Leica). Sections were preincubated overnight in PBS with 5% Triton
X-100 and 5% normal goat serum (NGS) and then incubated for 5 d at
4°C in PBS with 5% Triton X-100, 1% NGS, and anti-synapsin antibody
(1:50). The monoclonal anti-synapsin antibody was generated in mouse
against fusion proteins consisting of glutathione-S-transferase and the
Drosophila Syn1 protein (Klagges et al., 1996) kindly provided by Drs.
Erich Buchner and Christian Wegener (University of Würzburg). The
antibody labels synapse-rich neuropils in various insect species (Brandt
et al., 2005; Kurylas et al., 2008; Held et al., 2016). Following incubation
in anti-synapsin, sections were incubated in PBS with 5% Triton X-100,
1% NGS, and the secondary antibody (goat anti-mouse) conjugated with
Cy5 (1:300) for 3 d at 4°C. The sections were finally dehydrated in an
increasing ethanol series (as described above), cleared in methyl salicylate
(as described above), and mounted in Permount between two coverslips
(for a more detailed description of the protocol, see Heinze and
Homberg, 2008). Preparations were scanned with a confocal laser scanning microscope (Leica) using a DPSS laser (561 nm) for detection of
Cy3 and a He-Ne laser (633 nm) for detection of Cy5. Scans were visualized in AMIRA (version 5.4.5; FEI Visualization Sciences Group). Images were processed in Adobe Illustrator CC version 2017.1.0.
Preprocessing of physiological data. Recording traces were visualized
using Spike2. Action potentials were detected as events with a thresholdbased mechanism. The data were exported to a mat file. All subsequent
analysis was performed using custom functions written in MATLAB version 2017a (The MathWorks).
Experimental design and statistical analysis. For each stimulus presentation, a stimulus–response curve was obtained by calculating the mean
spiking activity in 10° bins. To assess the response of a neuron to a
stimulus condition, the stimulus–response curves of all presentations
were averaged. At least one clockwise and one counterclockwise rotation
of the polarizer/unpolarized light spot were averaged. Responses to
clockwise and counterclockwise tests were always pooled in equal numbers to avoid a shift in the preferred calculated angle due to rotation
direction. A directed modulation of spike rate by the orientation of the
E-vector or the azimuth of the unpolarized light spot was determined by
an angular–linear correlation analysis (Zar, 1999). The responsiveness of
the neuron to a stimulus was indicated by the significance (␣ ⫽ 0.05) of
the correlation coefficient (ral). The coefficient of determination (ral 2)
describes the strength of correlation between E-vector orientation or
light spot azimuth and the spike rate (for a detailed description, see Pegel
et al., 2018). To calculate the preferred E-vector or azimuth (⌽max), spike
times were transformed into angles by multiplying them with the stimulus rotation velocity. From these angles, the mean vector ⌽ was calculated (Batschelet, 1981) and defined as the preferred angle (⌽max). The
anti-preferred angle (⌽min) was defined as the angle 180° to ⌽max (azimuth tuning) or 90° to ⌽max (E-vector tuning). For all analyses of axial
data (E-vector stimulation), the angles were doubled (Batschelet, 1981).
Background activity was determined by selecting parts of the recording
without any stimulation and dividing them into 1 s bins. In each bin, the
spikes were counted. Spike counts were used to calculate the median
background activity. The correlation between the location of arborization in the PB and ⌽max was assessed by a circular–linear correlation
Pegel et al. • Two Compasses in the Central Complex
analysis as described by Kempter et al. (2012). The slice of PB arborization was used as the linear variable (values ranging from 0 to 15) and
⌽max as the circular variable. A linear regression model was fitted to the
circular–linear data by minimizing the circular error between measured
and predicted angles (Kempter et al., 2012). The slope of the regression
line was used to transform the linear variable into a circular one. Finally,
a circular correlation coefficient () was calculated. No prior assumptions on the data were necessary except for an estimate of the range of
reasonable slopes. It was determined as the slope ␣ of the regression line
with minimum mean circular distance from the data points within a
reasonable range of ⫾ 80° per PB column (Equation 1 in Kempter et al.,
2012). The circular–linear correlation coefficient is an analog to the Pearson’s product–moment correlation coefficient for linear–linear data, but
with higher validity when analyzing circular–linear associations
(Kempter et al., 2012).
For Figures 5 and 6, stimulus–response curves were smoothed. The
stimulus–response curve was normalized to the median background activity of the neuron. A smoothing spline was fitted onto the curve using
the MATLAB curve-fitting toolbox (smoothing parameter set to 10 ⫺4,
360 array elements). For experiments with different light spot elevations,
additional characteristics of the stimulus–response curve were calculated
using the smoothed stimulus–response curve: the tuning amplitude, and
the tuning width. The amplitude was determined by calculating the difference in normalized spike rate between the peak and the trough of the
fit curve. The tuning width was defined as the angular difference between
two points on the fit curve at half amplitude.
For experiments with combined stimulation (see Fig. 5), we calculated
the relative impact of the green and polarized light stimulation alone on
the response to simultaneous stimulation. We followed the assumption
that the tuning is constituted as follows:
f 共 ⌽ 兲 ⫽ p 共 ⌽ ⫹ 90 兲 䡠 w p ⫹ g 共 ⌽ 兲 䡠 w g ,
Where ⌽ is the stimulus angle in degrees, f is the hypothetic
tuning curve to simultaneous stimulation, p is the measured tuning curve to polarized light stimulation, g is the measured tuning
curve to green-light-spot stimulation, and wp兩g are the respective
weighting factors. Tuning curves were smoothed before calculations. We used an optimization approach to find the weighting
factors where the summed absolute difference between the measured response to simultaneous stimulation and f is minimal. To
this end, we used the MATLAB built-in function fminbnd to
minimize the function h共w兲 ⫽ 冱⌽ 兩c共⌽兲 ⫺ 共 p共⌽ ⫹ 90兲 䡠 w
⫹ g共⌽兲 䡠 wg 兲兩, where c is the measured tuning curve to simultaneous stimulation and w is the minimization parameter that
was constrained to the interval (⫺10, 10). In each optimization
iteration, wg was calculated by solving f for wg and inserting the
c⫺p 䡠 w
,
measured tuning curve for the hypothetic one: wg ⫽
g
where w is the optimization parameter of the current iteration. At
min(h(w)), the lowest absolute difference between measured and
hypothetic tuning curve w was taken as wp and wg was calculated
as above.
Results
Most of the data analyzed here are from recordings presented
previously (Pegel et al., 2018). In that study, we analyzed basic
response features of the neurons, including their tuning to the
plane of polarized light, unpolarized green and UV light spots,
response amplitudes, tuning widths, and tuning differences when
comparing clockwise and counterclockwise rotations of the stimuli. Here, we investigated whether morphological characteristics
of the neurons such as the innervated layer in the CBL and columnar domains in the PB, CBU, and CBL correspond to tuning
angles of the neurons in a topographic manner.
Pegel et al. • Two Compasses in the Central Complex
J. Neurosci., April 17, 2019 • 39(16):3070 –3080 • 3073
Figure 2. Morphology and physiology of TL neurons. A, B, Cy3 stainings of TL neurons (magenta). Neuropils were visualized by synapsin immunostaining (cyan). Scale bars, 20 m. A,
Arborizations of a TL2a neuron (top), a TL2b neuron (middle), and a TL3 neuron (bottom) in the CBL; single optical sections. B, Arborizations of the neurons in the lateral (LBU) or medial bulb (MBU);
projections of stacks of several optical sections. C, Circular histograms showing the average response of the TL2a (⌽max ⫽ 76°), TL2b (⌽max ⫽ 284°), and TL3 (⌽max ⫽ 133°) neuron presented in
A and B to a rotating green light spot (elevation ⫽ 45°). Green bars indicate mean spiking activity. Error bars indicate SD. Black circles indicate median background activity. n, Number of stimulus
presentations. D, Population vector averages for the green spot from the recorded TL2a, TL2b, and TL3 neurons (means from two or four green light spot rotations) indicated by their preferred
azimuth angle and vector length. Vector length ranges from 0 to unity (outer circle). Open dots indicate a preferred azimuth on the ipsilateral side; filled gray dots indicate a preferred angle on the
contralateral side. Asterisks indicate data from the histograms in C.
Tuning angles of TL neurons innervating different layers of
the CBL
To investigate whether azimuthal preference is topographically
represented in the layering of the CBL, we compared azimuthal
tuning and innervated layer in 10 recorded TL neurons. Six types
of tangential neuron termed TL1–TL6 have been distinguished in
the CBL of the locust (Müller et al., 1997; Bockhorst and
Homberg, 2015). We recorded from seven TL2 and three TL3
neurons, invading different layers of the CBL. Five TL2 neurons
invaded layer 2 of the CBL and are termed here TL2a (Fig. 2A).
Two TL2 neurons invaded layer 3 and are termed here TL2b (Fig.
2A). The three TL3 neurons arborized in layers 4 and 5 (Müller et
al., 1997). In the lateral bulb, TL2 and TL3 neurons receive signals
via microglomerular complexes (Träger et al., 2008). Whereas
TL2a neurons innervated microglomeruli in dorsal parts of the
lateral bulb, TL2b neurons innervated microglomeruli of more
ventral parts (Fig. 2B). In TL3 neurons, arborizations were located in the medial bulb, but one neuron additionally invaded a
few microglomeruli at the most mediodorsal tip of the lateral
bulb (Fig. 2B). All TL2 neurons responded with excitation at
⌽max and inhibition at ⌽min. TL3 neurons were generally tuned
only weakly to the azimuth of the green spot and either showed
exclusively excitation at ⌽max (Fig. 2C), inhibition at ⌽min, or
both. In all TL2a neurons, the preferred azimuth of the green light
spot was on the contralateral side (Fig. 2C,D). In contrast, in the
two TL2b neurons, the preferred azimuth was on the ipsilateral
side (Fig. 2C,D). The three TL3 neurons showed mixed responses, with ⌽max on the ipsilateral side in two neurons and on
the contralateral side in the third neuron (Fig. 2C,D).
Topographic representation of E-vector and azimuth angles
in the PB
In the PB, zenithal E-vectors are topographically represented,
indicating that the PB acts as an internal sky polarization compass
(Heinze and Homberg, 2007). This representation was found for
TB1, CPU1, CP1, and CP2 neurons (Heinze and Homberg,
2007), but not for CL1 neurons (Heinze and Homberg, 2009).
Here, we investigated whether a topographic representation of
azimuth of unpolarized light representing the sun is likewise
present across the PB. We recorded from CL1, TB1, CPU1, CP1,
and CP2 neurons and from an additional cell type, CPU2 neurons (Fig. 1A–C). Neurons were tested for E-vector coding by
presenting polarized light from the zenith and for azimuth coding by presenting an unpolarized green light spot rotating at an
3074 • J. Neurosci., April 17, 2019 • 39(16):3070 –3080
Pegel et al. • Two Compasses in the Central Complex
Figure 3. Internal representation of zenithal E-vector and azimuth in the CX. Preferred E-vector angles (A–C, blue markers) and preferred azimuth angles of the unpolarized light spot (Aⴕ,Bⴕ,Cⴕ,
green markers) are plotted against the slice of arborization of the respective neuron in the PB. Datasets are plotted three times: ⫾ 180° for preferred E-vectors and ⫾ 360° for preferred azimuths.
Data points are means from two or four stimulus presentations. n, Number of recordings. Solid lines show the best fit line only in cases in which 兩兩 ⬎ 0.25. Light spot elevation was at 45°. Aⴖ, Bⴖ,
Cⴖ, Mean E-vectors (blue double arrows) and mean azimuth angles (green arrows) taken from the circular–linear fits. For neurons arborizing additionally in the CB, the mean preferred angles were
transferred from the PB to the CB according to the wiring schemes as shown by Heinze and Homberg (2008). Wiring is indicated by shades of gray for the right brain hemisphere. Light gray indicates
unknown and thus hypothetical connections. A, Aⴕ, Preferred angles of CL1 neurons. Circular–linear regression shows low correlation between innervated slice and preferred angles for polarized
blue light ( ⫽ 0.11), but high correlation between innervated slice and unpolarized green light (y ⫽ ⫺42.5x ⫹ 202.9, ⫽ ⫺0.72). B, Bⴕ, Preferred angles of TB1 neurons. Correlation exists for
polarized blue light (y ⫽ 70.0 x ⫹ 81.5, ⫽ 0.60) but not for unpolarized green light ( ⫽ ⫺0.04). Due to similar location of varicose arborizations of TB1 neurons in the left and right PB
hemisphere, only data of the left hemisphere are shown. C, Cⴕ, Preferred angles of CPU1 neurons. Correlation exists for polarized blue light (y ⫽ 20.5x ⫹ 84.0, ⫽ 0.33) and unpolarized green light
(y ⫽ ⫺30.9x ⫹ 80.0, ⫽ ⫺0.54).
elevation of 45° around the animals’ head. The preferred angles
were plotted against the slice of arborization in the PB. Linear–
circular regression analysis was performed for CL1, TB1, and
CPU1 neurons because only for those cell types sample size was
above the critical limit of n ⫽ 10 recordings (Kempter et al.,
2012). Because TB1 neurons have varicose arborizations in one
PB slice of each brain hemisphere (separated by seven slices),
their preferred angles were plotted only for the left brain hemisphere. In CL1 neurons, the preferred E-vector was not correlated
with the slice of arborization in the PB (Fig. 3A). These observations are consistent with previous results (Heinze and Homberg,
2009). TB1 neurons showed a correlation between preferred
E-vector and slice of arborization (Fig. 3B). The data confirm the
results of Heinze and Homberg (2007), who showed a topographic representation of E-vectors for TB1 neurons. As in
Heinze and Homberg (2007), the fit line had a positive slope.
However, it covered an angular range of 490° across one PB hemisphere, substantially larger than the range of 169.4° found by
Heinze and Homberg (2007). In CPU1 neurons, the preferred
E-vector angle was highly correlated with the slice of arborization
in the PB (Fig. 3C), indicating an internal compass for zenithal
E-vectors in the PB (Fig. 3C⬙). The circular–linear fits covered an
angular range of 307° in CPU1 neurons from L8 to R8. The steepness of the fit line was again different from that of Heinze and
Pegel et al. • Two Compasses in the Central Complex
J. Neurosci., April 17, 2019 • 39(16):3070 –3080 • 3075
Homberg (2007), who reported a representation of 410° in CPU1
neurons.
A correlation between the slice of arborization in the PB and
the preferred azimuth of the unpolarized light spot was found in
CL1 and CPU1 neurons (Fig. 3A⬘,C⬘). This indicates that, in addition to the E-vector of polarized light, the azimuth of the green
spot is topographically represented in the PB (Fig. 3A⬙,C⬙). However, in contrast to the positive correlation between E-vector and
PB slice, the correlation between azimuth and PB arborization
was negative in both cell types. The fits for the preferred azimuth
of the unpolarized green spot covered 637.5° in CL1 neurons and
463.5° in CPU1 neurons across the PB. Because CPU1 and CL1
neurons arborize, not only in slices of the PB, but also in distinct
slices of the central body, we transferred the mean preferred angle
(i.e., the y-value of the fit line in the center of each slice) from the
PB to the CB following the wiring scheme of the respective neuron type (Heinze and Homberg, 2008). In CL1 neurons, mean
preferred azimuth angles resulted in a representation of 277.1°
across the CBL (Fig. 3A⬙). CPU1 neurons innervating PB slices
L6 –R6 arborize in two neighboring slices of the CBU. For the PB
slices L8, L7, R8, and R7, however, the wiring scheme is not
known because CPU1 neurons arborizing in these slices have
never been stained. By extrapolating the logic of connections
from the central slices, Heinze and Homberg (2008) suggested
that they invade corresponding outermost slices of the CBU. Following that scheme, mean E-vector angles across the eight double
slices covered a range of 127° in the CBU (Fig. 3C⬙). Interestingly,
the mean azimuth angles of CPU1 neurons in double slices of the
CBU were almost spatially opponent to each other except for
the outermost double slices, which were approximately parallel (Fig. 3C⬙).
Interaction between polarized and unpolarized light stimulus
Because the regression lines for preferred E-vectors and preferred
azimuth angles have different signs, we examined their relationship in more detail. First we investigated whether the angular
distance between the tuning to both stimuli (E-vector and green
azimuth) and the slice of arborization in the PB were correlated in
any way. Across all cell types, distances were independent of the
slice of PB arborization (Fig. 4) and varied greatly for a given slice.
In CL1 neurons, distances were widely dispersed from 0° to 180°
(Fig. 4A). In contrast, in most TB1 neurons, distances were
smaller than 90° (Fig. 4B). In CPU1 neurons, the distances clustered between 45° and 90° (Fig. 4C).
Second, to test for a possible cue preference or linear relationship between the two cues, we presented the unpolarized light
spot in combination with the zenithal E-vector (Fig. 5). The relative angle between both stimuli was set to 90°, corresponding to
the relationship between solar azimuth and zenithal E-vector in
the sky. We analyzed the responses of two TL2, three CL1, four
TB1, six CPU1, two CPU2, and one CP1 neuron. A hypothetical
response curve to combined stimulation was calculated to estimate the relative contribution of the single stimuli (E-vector or
light spot) to the measured response to combined stimulation.
The absolute values and the ratio of weighting factors varied from
neuron to neuron and could not be related to cell type, slice of PB
innervation, or distance between tuning to the E-vector and
green light spot. Weighting factors for azimuth tuning ranged
from 0 to 1.7 and, for E-vector tuning, from ⫺2 to 1.2. Their ratio
ranged from ⫺1.1 to 11.5. Some neurons showed a strong preference in the combined response for the E-vector or the unpolarized light spot, as shown for a CPU1 neuron preferring the
E-vector (Fig. 5A). Other cells showed a less pronounced prefer-
Figure 4. Distances between preferred E-vectors and azimuth angles. A–C, Distances between preferred E-vector angles and preferred azimuth angles (black markers) plotted against
the slice of arborization in the PB. Distances were calculated by subtracting the preferred
E-vector angle from the preferred azimuth of the green spot. Data points are means from two or
four stimulus presentations. A, Distances of CL1 neurons (n ⫽ 14). B, Distances of TB1 neurons
(n ⫽ 11). C, Distances of CPU1 neurons (n ⫽ 14).
ence for the E-vector, as shown for a CL1 and a TB1 neuron (Fig.
5 B, C), or for the azimuth of the green spot, as shown for a CPU1
and a CPU2 neuron (Fig. 5 D, E). Some neurons showed responses to combined stimulation, in which the contributions of
E-vector tuning and light spot tuning were similar (CL1; Fig. 5F ).
Nonetheless, in neurons not showing a strong preference, the
response to combined stimulation always revealed one higher
peak or one deeper trough in spike rate modulation across 360°.
Influence of elevation on azimuth tuning
In addition to its horizontal component (azimuth), the position
of the sun has a vertical component (elevation). Although only
3076 • J. Neurosci., April 17, 2019 • 39(16):3070 –3080
Pegel et al. • Two Compasses in the Central Complex
Figure 5. Responses to combined stimulation with polarized light and an unpolarized light spot. A–F, Tunings of individual CX neurons to the E-vector (blue), to the azimuth of the unpolarized
light spot (green), and to both stimuli presented simultaneously (black). Tunings are presented as smoothed stimulus–response curves based on means from four stimulus presentations except for
the E-vector data in C and F, which are means from two stimulus presentations. During combined stimulation, the E-vector was adjusted at 90° angular distance from the green light spot. Therefore,
the x-axis shows the orientation of the E-vector (blue) separated from the azimuth angle (green). Responses to the E-vector alone are shifted by 90°. Solid red lines show the best fit curves resulting
from summation of E-vector tuning and light spot tuning multiplied by a weighting factor wp for the E-vector tuning and wg for the azimuth tuning. Dotted lines indicate background activity. A,
Responses of a CPU1 neuron showing strong preference for the green light spot. E-vector tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 40°; p ⫽ 6 䡠 10 ⫺5. Azimuth tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 264°; p ⫽ 2䡠10 ⫺5. Combined response
bidirectional tuning: p ⫽ 0.83. Combined response unidirectional tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 249°; p ⫽ 1䡠10 ⫺3. B–F, Responses of CL1 neurons (B, F ), a TB1 neuron (C), a CPU1 neuron (D), and a CPU2 neuron
(E) to combined stimulation with less pronounced preference for the E-vector or the azimuth of the green spot. B, Responses of a CL1 neuron showing a preference for polarized light. E-vector tuning:
⌽max ⫽ 120°; p ⫽ 3䡠10 ⫺6. Azimuth tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 261°; p ⫽ 2䡠10 ⫺6. Combined response bidirectional tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 117°; p ⫽ 2䡠10 ⫺5. Combined response unidirectional tuning: p ⫽
0.64. C, Responses of a TB1 neuron. E-vector tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 39°; p ⫽ 2䡠10 ⫺5. Azimuth tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 285°; p ⫽ 4䡠10 ⫺5. Combined response bidirectional tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 36°; p ⫽ 2䡠10 ⫺4.
Combined response unidirectional tuning: p ⫽ 0.2. D, Responses of a CPU1 neuron. E-vector tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 152°; p ⫽ 2䡠10 ⫺7. Azimuth tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 69°; p ⫽ 8䡠10 ⫺7. Combined response
bidirectional tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 154°; p ⫽ 3䡠10 ⫺3. Combined response unidirectional tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 73°; p ⫽ 2䡠10 ⫺3. E, Responses of a CPU2 neuron. E-vector tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 99°; p ⫽ 4䡠10 ⫺3.
Azimuth tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 145°; p ⫽ 2䡠10 ⫺4. Combined response bidirectional tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 116°; p ⫽ 1䡠10 ⫺2. Combined response unidirectional tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 121°; p ⫽ 1䡠10 ⫺3. F,
Responses of a CL1 neuron. E-vector tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 104°; p ⫽ 2䡠10 ⫺5. Azimuth tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 92°; p ⫽ 5䡠10 ⫺3. Combined response bidirectional tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 119°; p ⫽ 1䡠10 ⫺3.
Combined response unidirectional tuning: ⌽max ⫽ 117°; p ⫽ 3䡠10 ⫺3.
the azimuth provides compass information, coding for solar elevation might provide daytime-dependent information and thus
could aid in time-compensated sun compass navigation. We
therefore tested different elevations of the unpolarized green light
spot in recordings of four CL1, four TB1, and six CPU1 neurons.
Only a few recordings showed an impact of elevation on the
azimuth tuning. One CL1 neuron showed a second peak in azimuth tuning when stimulated at low elevations (Fig. 6A). At
higher elevations of 50° or 60°, the second peak disappeared so
that the tuning was more directed toward ⌽max. Another phenomenon occurred in a CPU1 neuron: the tuning curve was flat
at high elevations and of higher amplitude at low elevations (Fig.
6B). Of all recordings, these two neurons showed the strongest
influence of elevation on azimuth tuning. The general effects on
the correlation strength of the tunings were small (Fig. 6C). Different elevations changed the significance of the correlation coefficient only in one CL1 (red), one TB1 (blue), and one CPU1
neuron (purple). All other neurons were either responsive to all
tested elevations (n ⫽ 10) or to none of them (n ⫽ 1). Elevations
different from 45° were often tested later in the recording so that
tuning parameters might have been altered by a change of neuronal background state. Across all cell types, the correlation
strength was most dispersed between elevations, whereas tuning
amplitude and width were only slightly affected. Nonetheless, no
common systematic change of tuning parameters occurred in any
neuron type.
Discussion
Side-specific azimuth representation in tangential inputs to
the CBL
In this study, we analyzed the relevance of solar azimuth and
elevation and of the zenithal E-vector for the internal representation of heading direction in locust CX neuropils. TL2 and TL3
neurons are the likely input elements of polarization and azimuth
information to the CX (Pegel et al., 2018). We identified two TL2
subtypes arborizing in different parts of the lateral bulb and different layers of the CBL (Müller et al., 1997) preferring a bright
light spot on either the ipsilateral (TL2a) or contralateral side
(TL2b) of the animal. Our findings are similar to characteristics
of ring neurons (R neurons; equivalent to locust TL neurons) of
Pegel et al. • Two Compasses in the Central Complex
J. Neurosci., April 17, 2019 • 39(16):3070 –3080 • 3077
Figure 6. Influence of stimulus elevation on tuning parameters of CL1, TB1, and CPU1 neurons. A, B, Smoothed stimulus–response curves of a CL1 neuron (A) and a CPU1 neuron (B) to rotation
of the green light spot presented at different elevations. Number of trials: n ⫽ 4 for all elevations in the CL1 neuron and the 45° elevation in the CPU1 neuron and n ⫽ 6 for all other elevations in
the CPU1 neuron. C, Tuning parameters of all recorded CL1, TB1, and CPU1 neurons. Tuning amplitude, tuning width, and correlation strength (ral 2) of the average stimulus–response curve of each
neuron plotted against the tested elevation. Colors code for individual neurons of the respective cell type. Open circles indicate data from significant responses; circles filled in gray indicate data from
nonsignificant responses. Data from the CL1 neuron coded in red are from the same neuron as data in A and data from the CPU1 neuron coded in dark blue are from the same neuron as data in B.
Number of trials ranged from n ⫽ 2 to n ⫽ 6 in all cell types.
Drosophila. R neurons arborizing in ventral parts of the bulb have
contralateral receptive fields for visual cues and innervate outer
layers of the ellipsoid body, whereas R neurons connecting microglomeruli in dorsal parts of the bulb to inner layers of the
ellipsoid body respond to ipsilateral targets (Shiozaki and Ka-
zama, 2017). The latter also encode the recent visual experience of
targets. These data suggest that different layers of the CBL/ellipsoid body not only receive input from different parts of the visual
field, but also different types of information related to memory
and decision making.
3078 • J. Neurosci., April 17, 2019 • 39(16):3070 –3080
Compass representations in CX slices
A compass-like representation of zenithal E-vectors in the PB has
been shown for TB1, CPU1, and CP1/CP2 neurons (Heinze and
Homberg, 2007). These findings are confirmed here for TB1 and
CPU1 neurons. As in Heinze and Homberg (2007), all regression
lines had a positive slope and, in CPU1 neurons, covered ⬃360°
of compass directions from L8 –R8. For TB1 neurons, the slope of
the regression line representing E-vectors differed from that
shown by Heinze and Homberg (2007), but this may be because
of the low sample size for TB1 neurons studied here. We found no
correlation between preferred E-vector and PB slice in CL1 neurons, confirming the findings of Heinze and Homberg (2009).
Although Heinze and Homberg (2007) calculated coefficients
using a linear–linear correlation analysis, we used a circular–linear approach, which affects the results in a negative way as follows: (1) with small sample size (n ⬍ 10), the correlation has only
low reliability (Kempter et al., 2012) and our sample size of TB1
neurons was just above this number and (2) for the analysis of
axial data such as the preferred E-vectors, the angles need to be
doubled to convert them into a circular variable. This artificially
increases the dispersion of data, thus decreasing the likelihood of
correlation or, in the case of correlation, its strength. Nonetheless, the circular–linear analysis is highly superior to a linear–
linear model. For the analysis of circularity, data points are
already present multiple times on the y-axis. This eliminates the
need to select the data points within the circular space to be used
for the linear–linear correlation analysis, which strongly influences its result.
In addition to E-vector topography, we found a topographical
representation of azimuth (green light spot) in CL1 and CPU1
neurons. In both cell types, the regression line had a negative
slope from L8 to R8, opposite to that for E-vector coding. It
covered a range of 638° in CL1 neurons. This range is close to the
twofold representation of 360° space of E-PG neurons (equivalents to locust CL1 neurons) in the fly (Green et al., 2017), which
was also a necessary assumption for a computational model of
path integration in bees (Stone et al., 2017). In contrast, the CPU1
columnar output neurons represented only 464° of azimuthal
directions. Because CPU1 neurons have so far not been found in
the two outermost columns (R7/8, L7/8) and also appear to be
absent in these columns in bees (Stanley Heinze, personal communication), this range might be further reduced to 340° from
R6 –L6. Being the output elements of the CX, CPU1 neurons, in
contrast to CL1 neurons, may not show a purely sensory signal
anymore, but rather a steering motor command signal. It is therefore reasonable that columnar outputs of the PB represent a range
of just 180° in one PB hemisphere because neurons from one
hemisphere have their presynaptic arborizations in the same lateral accessory lobe so that they might elicit turning behavior either to the left or to the right.
Elevation independence
In Drosophila, R neurons have approximately circular receptive
fields for a bright bar defined by a preferred azimuth and preferred elevation (Seelig and Jayaraman, 2013). In contrast, in the
locust, elevation of the green light spot was not encoded by PB
neurons because most neurons were either responsive to all elevations or to none. Whether this difference is species specific or
results from the fact that we did not collect data from TL neurons,
only from downstream columnar neurons that have not been
studied in Drosophila, remains to be established. The elevation
independence in the tuning to the green light suggests that only
the azimuth information is relevant for the coding properties of
Pegel et al. • Two Compasses in the Central Complex
the neurons studied here. Theoretically, information on solar
elevation could be useful for daytime estimation, but is apparently ignored in birds (Keeton, 1974; Wiltschko, 1980), honey
bees (von Frisch, 1965), and desert ants (Duelli and Wehner,
1973) in favor of a circadian clock input for time-compensated
sun compass orientation.
Biological significance
Honeybees, desert ants, and dung beetles are able to use both the
sun and the polarization pattern of the sky as navigational cues
(Brines and Gould, 1979; Wehner and Müller, 2006; el Jundi et
al., 2015). Both signals are processed in the CX. In addition to
encoding the plane of zenithal polarized light, CX neurons of
dung beetles (el Jundi et al., 2015) and monarch butterflies
(Heinze and Reppert, 2011) show azimuth-dependent responses
to a bright light spot assumed to represent the sun. In Drosophila,
azimuthal tunings to bright light spots are topographically represented in the ellipsoid body and PB (Seelig and Jayaraman,
2015; Turner-Evans et al., 2017). We show here that both the
representation of zenithal E-vectors and the azimuth of an unpolarized light spot are mapped to the PB slices in a compass-like
manner.
The two internal compasses for E-vector orientation and azimuth angle, however, do not support each other; they differ, not
only in their orientation, but also in the total angular range they
represent across the PB. Moreover, there is no 90° distance between preferred E-vector and preferred azimuth in any of the PB
slices, so that the natural distance between zenithal E-vector and
sun is not encoded. This raises the question of how the two compasses may interact in a natural setting. All data presented here
were obtained from animals harnessed tightly for intracellular
recordings. It is conceivable that the measured offset of the two
compasses may be altered by active locomotion and turning
movements of the locust. As shown in cockroaches (Varga and
Ritzmann, 2016) and flies (Turner-Evans et al., 2017), certain CX
neurons are tuned, not only to heading direction, but also to
turning velocity of the animal and even to turning history. Addressing these effects in locusts would likewise require recordings
during turning movements, ideally in freely moving animals.
Confinement of polarized light stimulation to a small area in
the zenith has probably also affected the E-vector representation
in the CX. Bees and ants need to see a sufficiently large part of the
sky polarization pattern to calculate solar position and make systematic errors when they observe only a small part of the blue sky
(Rossel and Wehner, 1984; Rossel, 1993; Wehner and Müller,
2006). Bech et al. (2014) showed that locust CX neurons encode
sky-like patterns of differently oriented E-vectors. If the receptive
fields of CX neurons are not zenith centered, then the internal
E-vector compass based on the zenithal E-vector may substantially differ from a polarization compass based on a complete
Rayleigh sky. Support for this hypothesis comes from a computational model of an insect-inspired polarization compass (Gkanias et al., 2018). Signals from a fan-shaped arrangement of
E-vector analyzers as present in the dorsal rim area of the eye were
fed into an array of compass neurons covering a 360° azimuth
range. When stimulating the E-vector analyzers with a rotating
polarizer instead of a Rayleigh pattern of E-vectors, the array of
compass neurons showed an ⬃180°-representation of directions
instead of 360° and, moreover, a paradox mirror-symmetric topography compared with Rayleigh sky stimulation. Unfortunately, stimulation with a pattern of polarizers mimicking the
Rayleigh sky is hardly possible in a laboratory setting. Therefore,
the polarization compass in the CX based on matched-filter
Pegel et al. • Two Compasses in the Central Complex
mechanisms for the Rayleigh sky may indeed be organized differently than revealed by the “single-polarizer” stimulation used
here.
In conclusion, our results support the assumption that CX
neurons are involved in navigation, not only by using a sky polarization compass, but also an azimuth compass possibly representing the sun. Both internal compasses emerge in neurons of
the PB and are present in columnar output neurons. Experiments
under the open sky are likely to reveal how both compasses interact with each other in a natural setting to produce a robust head
direction signal.
References
Batschelet E (1981) Circular statistics in biology. London: Academic.
Bech M, Homberg U, Pfeiffer K (2014) Receptive fields of locust brain neurons are matched to polarization patterns of the sky. Curr Biol
24:2124 –2129.
Bockhorst T, Homberg U (2015) Amplitude and dynamics of polarizationplane signaling in the central complex of the locust brain. J Neurophysiol
113:3291–3311.
Brandt R, Rohlfing T, Rybak J, Krofczik S, Maye A, Westerhoff M, Hege HC,
Menzel R (2005) Three-dimensional average-shape atlas of the honeybee brain and its applications. J Comp Neurol 492:1–19.
Brines ML, Gould JL (1979) Bees have rules. Science 206:571–573.
Brunner D, Labhart T (1987) Behavioural evidence for polarization vision
in crickets. Physiological Entomology 12:1–10.
Clements AN, May TE (1974) Studies on locust neuromuscular physiology
in relation to glutamic acid. J Exp Biol 60:673–705.
Collett TS (1992) Landmark learning and guidance in insects. Philos Trans
R Soc Lond B 337:295–303.
Dacke M, Nordström P, Scholtz CH (2003) Twilight orientation to polarized light in the crepuscular dung beetle Scarabaeus zambesianus. J Exp
Biol 206:1535–1543.
Duelli P, Wehner R (1973) The spectral sensitivity of polarized light orientation in Cataglyphis bicolor (Formicidae, Hymenoptera). J Comp Physiol
86:37–53.
el Jundi B, Pfeiffer K, Heinze S, Homberg U (2014a) Integration of polarization and chromatic cues in the insect sky compass. J Comp Physiol A
Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 200:575–589.
el Jundi B, Smolka J, Baird E, Byrne MJ, Dacke M (2014b) Diurnal dung
beetles use the intensity gradient and the polarization pattern of the sky
for orientation. J Exp Biol 217:2422–2429.
el Jundi B, Warrant EJ, Byrne MJ, Khaldy L, Baird E, Smolka J, Dacke M
(2015) Neural coding underlying the cue preference for celestial orientation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112:11395–11400.
Frost BJ, Mouritsen H (2006) The neural mechanisms of long distance animal navigation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16:481– 488.
Gkanias E, Risse B, Mangan M, Webb B (2018) From skylight input to behavioural output: a computational model of the insect polarised
light compass. Available at https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/
504597v1.
Gould JL (1998) Sensory bases of navigation. Curr Biol 8:R731–R738.
Green J, Adachi A, Shah KK, Hirokawa JD, Magani PS, Maimon G (2017) A
neural circuit architecture for angular integration in Drosophila. Nature
546:101–106.
Heinze S (2014) Polarized-light processing in insect brains: recent insights
from the desert locust, the monarch butterfly, the cricket, and the fruit fly.
In: Polarized light and polarization vision in animal sciences (Horváth G,
ed), pp 61–111. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer.
Heinze S, Homberg U (2007) Maplike representation of celestial E-vector
orientations in the brain of an insect. Science 315:995–997.
Heinze S, Homberg U (2008) Neuroarchitecture of the central complex of
the desert locust: intrinsic and columnar neurons. J Comp Neurol 511:
454 – 478.
Heinze S, Homberg U (2009) Linking the input to the output: new sets of
neurons complement the polarization network in the locust central complex. J Neurosci 29:4911– 4921.
Heinze S, Reppert SM (2011) Sun compass integration of skylight cues in
migratory monarch butterflies. Neuron 69:345–358.
Held M, Berz A, Hensgen R, Muenz TS, Scholl C, Rössler W, Homberg U,
Pfeiffer K (2016) Microglomerular synaptic complexes in the sky-
J. Neurosci., April 17, 2019 • 39(16):3070 –3080 • 3079
compass network of the honeybee connect parallel pathways from the
anterior optic tubercle to the central complex. Front Behav Neurosci
10:186.
Homberg U (2015) Sky compass orientation in desert locusts - evidence from
field and laboratory studies. Front Behav Neurosci 9:346.
Homberg U, Heinze S, Pfeiffer K, Kinoshita M, el Jundi B (2011) Central
neural coding of sky polarization in insects. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B
Biol Sci 366:680 – 687.
Keeton WT (1974) The orientational and navigational basis of homing in
birds. In: Recent advances in the study of behavior (Lehrman DS, Hinde
RA, Shaw E, eds), pp 47–132. New York: Academic.
Kempter R, Leibold C, Buzsáki G, Diba K, Schmidt R (2012) Quantifying
circular–linear associations: hippocampal phase precession. J Neurosci
Methods 207:113–124.
Klagges BR, Heimbeck G, Godenschwege TA, Hofbauer A, Pflugfelder GO,
Reifegerste R, Reisch D, Schaupp M, Buchner S, Buchner E (1996) Invertebrate synapsins: a single gene codes for several isoforms in Drosophila. J Neurosci 16:3154 –3165.
Kurylas AE, Rohlfing T, Krofczik S, Jenett A, Homberg U (2008) Standardized atlas of the brain of the desert locust, Schistocerca gregaria. Cell Tissue
Res 333:125–145.
Labhart T, Meyer EP (1999) Detectors for polarized skylight in insects: a
survey of ommatidial specializations in the dorsal rim area of the compound eye. Microsc Res Tech 47:368 –379.
Mappes M, Homberg U (2004) Behavioral analysis of polarization vision in
tethered flying locusts. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav
Physiol 190:61– 68.
Menzel R, Greggers U, Smith A, Berger S, Brandt R, Brunke S, Bundrock G,
Hülse S, Plümpe T, Schaupp F, Schüttler E, Stach S, Stindt J, Stollhoff N,
Watzl S (2005) Honey bees navigate according to a map-like spatial
memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:3040 –3045.
Merlin C, Heinze S, Reppert SM (2012) Unraveling navigational strategies
in migratory insects. Curr Opin Neurobiol 22:353–361.
Müller M, Homberg U, Kühn A (1997) Neuroarchitecture of the lower division of the central body in the brain of the locust (Schistocerca gregaria).
Cell Tissue Res 288:159 –176.
Ofstad TA, Zuker CS, Reiser MB (2011) Visual place learning in Drosophila
melanogaster. Nature 474:204 –207.
Pegel U, Pfeiffer K, Homberg U (2018) Integration of celestial compass cues
in the central complex of the locust brain. J Exp Biol 221:jeb171207.
Pfeiffer K, Kinoshita M, Homberg U (2005) Polarization-sensitive and
light-sensitive neurons in two parallel pathways passing through the anterior optic tubercle in the locust brain. J Neurophysiol 94:3903–3915.
Reppert SM, Guerra PA, Merlin C (2016) Neurobiology of monarch butterfly migration. Annu Rev Entomol 61:25– 42.
Rossel S (1993) Navigation by bees using polarized skylight. Comp Biochem
Physiol A 104:695–708.
Rossel S, Wehner R (1984) How bees analyse the polarization patterns in the
sky: experiments and model. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural
Behav Physiol 155:605– 613.
Schmeling F, Wakakuwa M, Tegtmeier J, Kinoshita M, Bockhorst T, Arikawa
K, Homberg U (2014) Opsin expression, physiological characterization
and identification of photoreceptor cells in the dorsal rim area and main
retina of the desert locust, Schistocerca gregaria. J Exp Biol 217:3557–3568.
Seelig JD, Jayaraman V (2013) Feature detection and orientation tuning in
the Drosophila central complex. Nature 503:262–266.
Seelig JD, Jayaraman V (2015) Neural dynamics for landmark orientation
and angular path integration. Nature 521:186 –191.
Shiozaki HM, Kazama H (2017) Parallel encoding of recent visual experience and self-motion during navigation in Drosophila. Nat Neurosci 20:
1395–1403.
Stone T, Webb B, Adden A, Weddig NB, Honkanen A, Templin R, Wcislo W,
Scimeca L, Warrant E, Heinze S (2017) An anatomically constrained
model for path integration in the bee brain. Curr Biol 27:3069 –3085.e11.
Träger U, Wagner R, Bausenwein B, Homberg U (2008) A novel type of
microglomerular synaptic complex in the polarization vision pathway of
the locust brain. J Comp Neurol 506:288 –300.
Turner-Evans D, Wegener S, Rouault H, Franconville R, Wolff T, Seelig JD,
Druckmann S, Jayaraman V (2017) Angular velocity integration in a fly
heading circuit. Elife 6:e23496.
Varga AG, Ritzmann RE (2016) Cellular basis of head direction and contextual cues in the insect brain. Curr Biol 26:1816 –1828.
3080 • J. Neurosci., April 17, 2019 • 39(16):3070 –3080
Varga AG, Kathman ND, Martin JP, Guo P, Ritzmann RE (2017) Spatial
navigation and the central complex: sensory acquisition, orientation, and
motor control. Front Behav Neurosci 11:4.
von Frisch K (1949) Die Polarisation des Himmelslichtes als orientierender
Faktor bei den Tänzen der Bienen. Experientia 5:142–148.
von Frisch K (1965) Tanzsprache und Orientierung der Bienen. Berlin/
Heidelberg/New York: Springer.
Wehner R (1984) Astronavigation in insects. Annu Rev Entomol 29:
277–298.
Wehner R, Müller M (2006) The significance of direct sunlight and polarized skylight in the ant’s celestial system of navigation. Proc Natl Acad Sci
U S A 103:12575–12579.
Pegel et al. • Two Compasses in the Central Complex
Weir PT, Dickinson MH (2012) Flying Drosophila orient to sky polarization. Curr Biol 22:21–27.
Wiltschko R (1980) Die Sonnenorientierung der Vögel. I. Die Rolle der
Sonne im Orientierungssystem und die Funktionsweise des Sonnenkompasses. J Ornithol 121:121–143.
Wystrach A, Graham P (2012) View-based matching can be more than image matching: the importance of considering an animal’s perspective.
Iperception 3:547–549.
Zar JH (1999) Biostatistical analysis, Ed 4. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice
Hall.
Zars T (2009) Spatial orientation in Drosophila. J Neurogenet 23:104 –
110.