Neural Systems Control of Spatial Orienting
M. I. Posner; Y. Cohen; R. D. Rafal
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, Vol.
298, No. 1089, The Neuropsychology of Cognitive Function. (Jun. 25, 1982), pp. 187-198.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0080-4622%2819820625%29298%3A1089%3C187%3ANSCOSO%3E2.0.CO%3B2-%23
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences is currently published by The Royal
Society.
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/about/terms.html. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained
prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in
the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.
Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/journals/rsl.html.
Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.
The JSTOR Archive is a trusted digital repository providing for long-term preservation and access to leading academic
journals and scholarly literature from around the world. The Archive is supported by libraries, scholarly societies, publishers,
and foundations. It is an initiative of JSTOR, a not-for-profit organization with a mission to help the scholarly community take
advantage of advances in technology. For more information regarding JSTOR, please contact
[email protected].
http://www.jstor.org
Sun Jan 6 17:27:27 2008
Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 298, 187-198 (1982)
[ 187 ]
Printed in Great Britain
Neural systems control of spatial orienting
M. I. P O S N E RY.
, C O H E NA N D R. D. R A F A L
Department of Psychology, University of Oregon Eugene, Oregon 97403, U.S.A.
and Neurology Department, Good Samaritan Hospital, Portland, Oregon 97210, U.S.A.
A peripheral visual cue in a n empty field (1) often summons head or eyes, or both, ( 2 )
improves efficiency at the cued position while attention is directed to it, even without
overt movements, and ( 3 ) reduces processing efficiency at the cued position once
attention is withdrawn. We have studied the time course and the effects of mid-brain
and cortical damage on these components of orienting.
The facilitation arises from shifts in covert attention. In cases of mid-brain degeneration due to progressive supranuclear palsy, saccadic movements were abolished, while
covert orienting still occurs. However, covert orienting was found to be delayed in
directions in which eye movements were most affected, suggesting a role for mid-brain
pathways in covert orienting. Parietal lesions can cause massive loss in detection
contralateral to the lesion. This is especially true when attention has been directed to
the opposite side. These findings relate aspects of covert orienting of attention to neural
control systems.
Perhaps the simplest of all cognitive acts involves the orienting of attention toward an event.
We can direct attention to facilitate the processing of information presented at the sensory surface
or of material stored in memory. Such acts are cognitive in the sense that they involve an active
organism responding to environmental events in a way that is largely lost when the organism is
asleep or anaesthetized. In the last several years we (Posner 1980; Posner & Cohen 1980b) have
been studying the act of orienting by normal humans to visual stimuli occurring in a blank field
in a n effort to explore the component processes involved in spatial orienting. Our hope is that
a fundamental grasp of these component processes will allow the establishment of useful links
to underlying neural systems.
Current work in neuroscience (Mountcastle 1978; Wurtz et al. 1980) suggests that cells in the
superior colliculus are of special importance in overt shifts of attention, while parietal lobe cells
are involved more generally when attention is shifted to a visual object even without eye movements. In this paper we first outline an experimental method that has been used in normal
human subjects to study the components of covert orienting and then examine the effects of
midbrain and cortical damage by applying this method to populations of patients.
ATTENTIOM
NO V E M E N T S
I n our experiments, the presentation of a central or peripheral cue indicating the likely location of a target produces a movement of attention in the direction of the cued location. We
distinguish overt components of orienting such as postural changes or eye movements from a
covert change of orienting that involves central attentional systems. Covert attention movements can be inferred by changes in the efficiency of target detection at the cued location in
comparison with other locations in the visual field. Efficiency has been measured by changes in
188
M. I. P O S N E R , Y. C O H E N A N D R. D. R A F A L
reaction time to report the presence of the target signal (Posner 1980), by the probability of
detecting near-threshold stimulus events (Bashinski & Bachrach 1980; Remington 1980) or by
changes in electrical activity induced by the target (Goldberg & Wurtz 1972). We have indexed
these changes of efficiency in a variety of experimental situations that involve either simple
detection of the presence of a luminance change or the recognition ofthe semantic category ofthe
visual event (Posner 1978, 1980).
detection
stimulus
)
FIGURE 1. Paradigm for studying covert orienting i n normal subjects. Three boxes are always present i n the visual
field. Subjects fixate o n the centre box. A t the start o f the trial, one o f the two peripheral boxes brightens (cue).
After a specified interval a detection signal is presented i n one o f the boxes.
The changes in efficiency that we observe are sufficiently time-locked to cues that it seems
reasonable to call them movements. For a peripheral visual cue 7" from the fovea we find that it
takes approximately 50 ms for the changes in efficiency at the target location to begin and that
there is a marked improvement in the efficiency at the target location over the first 150 ms after
the cue.
This time-locking is shown quite exquisitely in a recent study. The basic experimental arrangement is shown in figure 1. Subjects fixated the central box (eye position was monitored by
e.0.g.) and their task was to press a single key whenever a bright detection stimulus (target)
appeared. Each trial began with a cue which was the brightening of one of the two peripheral
boxes. I n one condition the target appeared on the side that was brightened (cued side) 80 yoof
the time and on the side opposite to the cue 20 O/b of the time. In a second condition the target
was on the side opposite to the cue 80 % of the time and the cued side 20 Oj, of the time.
Results are shown in figure 2. The peripheral cue serves to summon attention in both conditions, leading to an initial advantage for the cued side. This occurs whether or not the target is
likely to fall on the same side as the cue. However, in the condition where the target is unlikely
to appear on cued side (triangles) the subject shifts attention to the side opposite the cue, showing an advantage there 300 ms after the cue. I t might be suspected that the initial advantage to
the cued side was due to brightness enhancement or some other purely sensory factor but we can
show this is not so by using a peripheral cue involving dimming of the box or by the use of a
purely central cue (Posner & Cohen 19806 ) .
The shifts of attention first toward the cue and then to the expected side are sufficiently timelocked that they can be observed over the 500 ms interval between cue and target. I t has also
been shown that probe events that occur with low probability at positions through which attention is passing become more efficient at a time intermediate between the cue and maximum
C O N T R O L O F SPATIAL O R I E N T I N G
189
facilitation at the end point for the attention movement (Shulman et al. 1979). This result
suggests a n analogue movement of facilitation from fixation toward the target position. A
related finding has also suggested that attention moves between locations within a visual image
in an analogue fashion (Pinker 1980). Figure 2 also illustrates that attention can be drawn from
an external source or can be moved in accordance with internally generated expectations. Thus,
the cue seems to draw attention toward a location even if there is a low probability of finding a
target there, but the expectation of a target also has a powerful control over orienting.
1
1
cue
1
1
I
I
1
FIGURE
2. Reaction time for reporting the presence of the detection signal as a function of the interval (milliseconds) between cue and detection signal. The circles refer to a condition in which the detection stimulus is on
the cued side with probability 0.8 and on the side opposite to the cue with probability 0.2. The triangles refer
to a condition in which the detection stimulus is on the cued side with probability 0.2 and the side opposite to
the cue with probability 0.8. Solid line, cued side; broken line, side opposite to the cue.
We have shown in separate experiments that attention will be drawn toward the cued side
even when all locations are equally probable (Posner & Cohen I 980 6 ) . When an eye movement
is summoned by a peripheral cue, attention moves to the cue approximately 200 ms before the
eyes begin to move (Posner 1980). However, attention movements are not an inevitable consequence of eye movement. When the subjects can pre-programme changes of the eyes, they may
do so without a shift of attention and indeed may move attention in the opposite direction of an
eye movement (Posner I 980). This constellation of relations between covert orienting and eye
movement provides evidence of a functional relation between the two systems, but one that can
be decoupled. The systems can operate independently but, like hand and eye, usually tend to
operate in close coordination. No doubt this learning occurs very early in life (Posner &
Rothbart 1981) as attentional systems mature and come to control various peripheral devices
such as hand and eye.
Recently, we have reported (Posner & Cohen 19806) that a peripheral cue produces two
opposed effects upon the information processing system. O n the one hand, it summons a movement of attention that, as described previously, enhances efficiency of processing information at
the target location. O n the other hand, it also produces a temporary inhibition. This can be seen
quite well in figure 2. At 500 ms the facilitation of a probable event on the side opposite to the
cue is much greater than the facilitation of an equally probable event that occurs on the cued
side. This appears to be due to the inhibition that arises when sensory information is used to
summon attention.
190
M. I. P O S N E R , Y. C O H E N A N D R. D. R A F A L
Because the facilitation that we have described occurs from central as well as from peripheral
cues, we call it attentional and see it as the result of orienting or aligning attention to a visual
location. The inhibition effect, however, seems to depend upon the presentation of sensory
information in the periphery'because it does not arise when a central cue is presented. Because
facilitation and inhibition have opposed influences upon the efficiency of information processing,
tracing the time course of efficiency following a peripheral cue can only tell us about the net
results of the orienting of attention and sensory inhibition. T o observe a more purely inhibitory
affect we summon attention away from the cued event at some time after the initial cue and
then compare the efficiency of performance at the cued location with other locations of equal
eccentricity in the visual field. Under these circumstances, it is clear that detection of the cued
location is inhibited with respect to other locations in the visual field. There is a balance between
the orienting of attention, on the one hand, and the sensory inhibition initiated by the peripheral
cue, on the other. Perhaps this balance tends to prevent overcommitment of the subject's
resources to a cued position and is in line with the general tendency of the nervous system to use
opposed systems to maintain equilibrium.
The act of orienting attention to a location in visual space becomes more complex as we
analyse its components experimentally. We are clearly not yet finished with the delineation of
the components of orienting in normal subjects, even in the visual modality and less so in other
sensory systems. Yet it seems important to attempt to link the results that we have so far to
clinical and neuroscience observations. In particular, work in the alert monkey shows that cells
in the superior colliculus, whose receptive field is in the neighbourhood of a target for an eye
movement, are selectively enhanced well before the eyes begin to move (Goldberg & Wurtz
1972). Moreover, both clinical (Weinstein & Friedland 1977) and neuroscience observations
(Mountcastle 1978; Robinson et al. 1980) argue that cells in the parietal lobe are closely related
to orienting of visual attention, whether it be overt or covert. If these systems are indeed related
to the mechanisms that we have been describing, it should be possible to observe their disruption
in cases of insults to mid-brain and cortical structures. For the last year, we (Rafal et al. 1981 ;
Walker et al. 1982) have been engaged in such an effort.
T o further our understanding of the relation between movements of the eyes and covert
facilitation, we have studied patients who suffer from a form of Parkinsonism called progressive
supranuclear palsy (PSP) (Goodale 1980; Rafal & Grimm 1981; Rafal et al. 1981). I n addition
to the clinical and pathological abnormalities seen in Parkinson's disease, in PSP the superior
colliculu~,pretectum, periaqueductal grey and mesencephalic raphe degenerate, and clinically
there is a distinctive opthalmoplegia with loss of voluntary saccades while vestibulo-ocular
reflexes remain intact. Further, the oculomotor deficit is characteristically more severe in the
vertical than in the horizontal plane; thus in some of these patients it is possible to compare
performance in dimensions in which the eyes cannot be moved voluntarily with dimensions that
show less severe impairment. T o provide confidence that we could link any deficits found in
covert orienting specifically to the mesencephalic lesions causing the opthalmoplegia (rather to
some other clinical or pathologic abnormality that PSP shares with Parkinson's disease), we also
studied four Parkinson's disease patients as controls, both off and on medication, in the same
experiments.
CONTROL O F SPATIAL ORIENTING
191
In our first experiment (PSP no. 1) we studied three PSP patients. In this study we provided a
long lasting visual cue (500 ms) followed by a target to which the subject has to respond. Cues
were a l ohexagon presented So above or below fixation in the vertical condition and 10" to the
left or right of fixation in the horizontal condition. The target remained present for 1 s after
presentation and we waited 5 s for a response before proceeding to the next trial. The target was
presented on the cued side on 80 % of the trials (valid trials) and on the side opposite to the cue
in 20 % of the trials (invalid trials). Details of the intervals between cue and probe are shown in
figure 3. Each subject was run for several sessions of approximately 200 trials in each condition
(horizontal and vertical).
time course/ms
50
probe:
probe:
50
150
150
300
500
550
FIGURE
3. Paradigm used for the study of mid-brain and cortical patients. In PSP no. 1 the cue was left present in
the field for 500 ms before presenting any detection stimuli (probes). In PSP no. 2 the detection stimuli
(probes) are presented at short and long intervals following the cue onset.
The results of this study were simple and clearcut. Reaction times differed greatly between the
three subjects ranging from speeds that would be roughly normal for this age range (500 ms)
to reaction times more than twice normal. In general reaction times were slightly faster in the
vertical than in the horizontal direction, but this result turned out to be accounted for by the
differences in the eccentricities of the targets and appear to be equivalent when the same
eccentricity is used. Each subject showed a clear advantage of the cued side over the side opposite
to the cue a t the earliest probe position studied that tended to decline over the interval (see
figure 4). The initial advantage of the cued side was significant in each patient, and there was
no overall interaction between direction (vertical against horizontal) and the magnitude of the
advantage of the cued side. The results showed quite clearly that patients unable to make
saccades in the vertical direction could still move covert attention in that direction. A more
detailed analysis of the data of each patient suggested that there were longer reaction times when
the cue was presented in the direction in which eye movements were relatively intact followed by
a target in the direction in which eye movements were severely impaired. This suggested to us
that there might be impairments in the latency of covert orienting in directions that showed the
strongest eye movement problems.
For this reason we designed a second study (PSP no. 2) in which we presented a peripheral
cue for 300 ms followed by targets occurring 50, 150, 550 and 1000 ms after the cue's onset
(see figure 3). This allowed us to compare the speed of covert orienting in the horizontal and
vertical direction. Otherwise the study was the same as outlined above. Four patients were
192
M. I. P O S N E R , Y. C O H E N A N D R. D. R A F A L
studied, one of whom had been in the original experiment. The results of this study are somewhat
more complex than in the previous study in that different patients had different patterns of eye
movement and covert orienting deficits. The overall results (see figure 5) indicate that covert
orienting was quite rapid in the horizontal direction. An advantage of the cued side is present at
50 ms and remains present over the entire interval. In the vertical direction there is no clear
advantage of the cued side over the opposite side until 1000 ms after the cue.
v a l i d
- - - invalid
I
n
',
'A- -- - -- -- -
horizontal
_--0
FIGURE4. Reaction time as function of the interval between cue and detection stimulus (probe) presented in the
vertical and horizontal dimensions. The valid side (solid lines and symbols) is the one that had the cue and on
which 0.8 of the detection stimuli occur. Data from PSP no. 1. Bars indicate one standard error.
FIGURE5 . Reaction time as a function of the interval between cue and detection stimulus (probe) for stimulus
presented in the vertical (circles) and horizontal (triangles) dimensions. The valid side (solid lines and
symbols) is that the one that had the cue and on which 0.8 of the detection stimuli occur. Data are from PSP
no. 2.
CONTROL O F SPATIAL ORIENTING
193
I t is instructive to examine the one patient who had been in both studies and whose reaction
times were quite short. She has a complete inability to move her eyes voluntarily in the vertical
direction. She showed some slowing of rightward horizontal movement, but leftward movements
were unimpaired as far as we could tell. Her reaction time results indicated a clear advantage of
the cued side by 150 ms in the horizontal direction, but not until 550 ms in the vertical direction
In the vertical direction she showed a disadvantage on the cued side at 50 and 150 ms. We also
used this subject in an experiment with a central rather than a peripheral cue. Once again she
showed much faster e f f e c ~of the cue in the horizontal than in the vertical direction. In this
patient there was no doubt that she could orient to cues in either direction, since she had
demonstrated this in the first experiment. However, there were systematically longer times
required to demonstrate covert orienting in the direction in which saccades were most affected.
I t is useful to compare the data of the PSP patients with those obtained from four Parkinson
patients (Walker e t al. 1982) in the same task. The Parkinson patients had rather short reaction
times generally and were similar in overall level of performance to the fastest PSP patient.
However, the Parkinson patients show an advantage of the cued over the side opposite to the
cue by 50 ms after input. The data of these four patients are quite similar to those found in
normal young subjects. Neither the normals nor the Parkinson patients show any difference
between covert orienting in the vertical and horizontal directions. Moreover, the Parkinson
patients show no difference in the speed of covert orienting whether they are medicated or not.
This finding suggests that the substantial impairment in mid-brain damage found in common
between PSP and Parkinsonism does not contribute to the deficit in spatial orienting found in
PSP patients. This result and the difference found in PSP patients between the two directions of
orienting suggest that their problem is related to the unique part of the PSP syndrome that
results in impairment of the eye movement system. Presumably this relates to the mesencephalic
lesions that produce the distinctive ophthalmoplegia in the PSP patients.
Another result favouring a role for mid-brain mechanisms in the control of covert orienting in
human beings has been reported by using somewhat similar techniques by Holtzman et al.
(1981). Their patients had surgery that disconnected the two cerebral hemispheres. When either
hemisphere was provided information about the place that a letter or digit would be presented,
reaction time improved to the cued position and slowed for the uncued positions. This occurred
irrespective of whether the target was given to the same hemisphere that had received the cue
or to the opposite hemisphere. Since these studies used a long time between cue and target, we
do not know how rapid the speed of covert orienting is in these patients.
We have summarized observations that implicate mid-brain pathways in the control of covert
visual orienting. The data from PSP patients indicate that mid-brain damage specific to their
ability to make saccades interferes with the latency of covert orienting. Despite the widespread
character of the lesions in these patients the difference between the vertical and horizontal
dimensions and the lack of interference with covert orienting in Parkinson's patients both
suggest that the effects of covert orienting latency are from the specific systems that are involved
with vertical eye movements. The role of mid-brain systems in aspects of orienting is supported
by findings from patients with commissurotomy (Holtzman e t al. 1981) and from blind sight
(see Weiskrantz 1980).
However, even the extensive damage to voluntary eye movement found in PSP patients does
not abolish their covert orienting ability even in the vertical direction. This fact suggests that
other pathways are also important in the control of covert orienting. Such a conclusion also
13
Vol. 298. B
194
M. I. P O S N E R , Y. C O H E N A N D R . D. R A F A L
follows from important dissociations between eye movement control and the control of covert
orienting that we have found in normals. For example, eye movements are more heavily
influenced by temporal than by nasal visual input (Posner & Cohen 1980a), but the temporal
field has no similar dominance over temporal order judgements (Posner & Cohen 198oa) or the
direction of covert orienting (Shulman 1979). These findings suggest important dissociations
between eye movement systems and those controlling covert orienting and conscious awareness
of the stimulus event.
I t has long been known that parietal lesions will sometimes lead to an inability to be aware of
a stimulus opposite the lesion if it is presented at the same time as an event ipsilateral to the
lesion (extinction) (Weinstein & Friedland 1977). Studies on alert monkeys have suggested that
cells in area 7 are enhanced when the monkey attends to a stimulus even if he does not move his
eyes toward it (Robinson et al. 1980). These studies have led to the idea that area 7 may be
related to important aspects of spatial attention (Mountcastle 1978; Wurtz et al. 1980).
We have examined six patients suffering from insult to the right parietal lobe due either to
vascular or to tumour processes. Five of these six patients were diagnosed clinically as having
extinction and visual neglect without hemianopsia. We have attempted to determine the relation
between the lesions and these clinical observations.
Our experiments were conducted in the same way as described before (see figure 3 ) . Our
results are preliminary but they do suggest very close relationships between the clinical syndrome
and the attentional orienting mechanisms that we have been discussing in this paper. Since
valid trials occurred 80 % of the time, the results for these trials are most complete. For five
patients, information going directly to the undamaged hemisphere is faster at all intervals
studied than information going directly to the damaged hemisphere. The one exception was the
patient who was not diagnosed as having visual neglect.
For two of our patients, the data are complete enough to compare valid and invalid trials.
Consider the first patient, R.S., who suffered from a tumour to the right parietal lobe which on
computed tomography scan appeared to involve most of the right parietal lobe. The optic radiations and thalamic area were spared.
His data are shown in figure 6. For valid trials (solid lines) this patient showed a small but
consistent advantage to targets occurring on the side ipsilateral to the lesion over those occurring
contralateral to the lesion. The time between the cue and the target did not affect this relation
strongly suggesting that the results were not mediated by overt eye movements. We cannot be
certain of this, however, because it is very difficult to determine the degree to which the eyes are
correctly fixated in these patients.
We can first examine the relation between valid and invalid trials where the target occurs
ipsilateral to the lesion. There is a clear validity effect that emerges 550 ins after the cue. This is
relatively slow compared with a validity effect in normal subjects. The validity effect is of a size
that approximates normal.
The results are stirkingly different when targets are presented on the side contralateral to
the damaged hemisphere. After a valid cue, reaction times are only somewhat longer than
those after a valid cue to the undamaged side, but when attention is to the non-neglected field
and a target occurs on the neglected side, there is massive interference with the processing
efficiency of the target information. At short intervals, the interference is so great that the patient
C O N T R O L OF SPATIAL O R I E N T I N G
195
usually does not detect the target within the 5 s allowed. This represents a confirmation of the
classical extinction phenomena where on double simultaneous stiniulation the subject is unable
to report information contralateral to the lesion. However, even with intervals of 1000 ms, so
that the cue has been gone from the field for more than 700 ms, there is still a massive interference with reaction time.,R.S. generally detects the presence of the target, but he is extremely
slow in doing so.
FIGURE
6. Median reaction time for valid (solid lines) and invalid (broken lines) trials for one right parietal patient
(R.S.). Bars indicate one standard error from the median. The Y axis is broken between 600 and 1000 ms to
allow data to be shown conveniently. A median reaction time of 5000 ms indicates that most targets were
not responded to within the allowed interval. @, 0 , Left target; A , A, right target.
Let us now examine the other patient, not diagnosed as suffering from visual neglect but who
had a form of extinction on double simultaneous stimulation in the tactile domain. Valid trials
for this patient showed no difference between the damaged and undamaged side in reaction
time. However, like R.S., an invalid trial in which the target is presented to the side opposite the
lesion produces a very powerful interference effect. When the interval is short, many targets are
missed and the interference shows a very gradual reduction with this interval but still remains
present to the longest time studied.
Of particular interest is a finding that we made in patient R.S. that has not yet been documented in other patients. In this condition, we used a central cue that was an arrow at fixation
extending less than $" across the mid-line with the arrowhead pointing either to the right or
left of fixation. The patient is instructed to orient attention in the direction of the arrow because
0.8 of the targets occurred on the cued site. For valid trials we get similar results to those obtained
with the peripheral cue. He was faster on the side ipsilateral to the lesion that contralateral to
the lesion. For invalid trials, we also get the same result as obtained with the peripheral cue.
Thus, when the physical event is a central arrow that instructs orienting rather than a stimulus
196
M. I. P O S N E R , Y. C O H E N A N D R . D. R A F A L
on the contralateral side, one still obtains evidence for extinction. This result suggests that
extinction does not depend upon the physical cue but on the way in which it directs central
attention.
These results are still fragmentary but they do suggest important relations between damage at
the parietal lobe and covert attention as we have measured it. According to the theory that has
been developed from the study of normals, the delay in processing an event on the uncued side is
due to a slowing in the time for that event to enter a general attentional system responsible for
detecting the event. The fact that under conditions where the uncued event is slowest one can
find a complete inability to be aware of the event in some parietal patients provides support for
the idea that such latency shifts are a sign of systems responsible for awareness in normal
persons. They also confirm many findings documented in the clinical literature but add
quantitative details to them and provide a better basis for comparison with mid-brain patients
and normal persons than has been available through clinical observation.
These studies have attempted to dissect the simple act of cognitive orienting into a set of timelocked facilitatory and inhibitory components. We have been able to show that some of these
components are affected by brain injury at mid-brain and cortical levels. The details of midbrain and cortical effects are different. At the level of the mid-brain, only the latency of covert
orienting seems to be affected. Targets are uniformly detected and there is no evidence of
extinction from a misdirection of attention. Patients are able to orient attention in directions to
which they cannot move their eyes voluntarily. None the less, the latency changes we found are
accompanied by certain clinically observable changes in the natural orienting behaviour of
these patients (Rafal & Grimm 1981). For example, they often fail to turn spontaneously toward
individuals who approach them, even though they may be quite able to do so on command.
I n contrast, the parietal lesions produce a massive loss of information from the field opposite
the lesions in cases where the subject's attention is directed away from the target. Extinction to
double simultaneous stimulation may be seen as a limiting condition in which attentional shifts
occur in close temporal proximity to the target event. I t seems to be the attentional shift and not
the simultaneous occurrence of the cue and target that produces extinction.
Experiments have shown quite clearly that the orienting mechanisms described in this paper
are important in high-level cognitive activity. For example, Chang (1981) was able to show that
such covert orienting mechanisms are intimately involved in increasing the sensitivity of the
visual field to the right of fixation during reading normal English and to the left when English is
read upside-down. Treisman & Gelade (1980) have argued for the importance of such covert
mechanisms in tying together features of visual objects. Pinker (1980) believes similar covert
orienting effects to be involved in our attending to parts of the three-dimensional visual field
and to memory images of such fields. Thus, from the cognitive point of view, attentional orienting has been shown to be a n important feature of complex human activity.
The experiments that we describe here seem to be capable of linking such orienting mechanisms to underlying neural systems at both mid-brain and cortical levels. Our results suggest that
both mid-brain and cortical sites are important in the efficient execution of covert orienting.
Taking our results together with those of Goldberg & Wurtz (1972), it appears that when tasks
involve eye movement, attention produces facilitation at the level of the superior colliculus.
C O N T R O L O F SPATIAL O R I E N T I N G
197
Although Goldberg & Wurtz are unable to find evidence for selective enhancement when the
task does not involve eye movements, it is still possible that mid-brain systems affect the ability
of higher centres to shift attention to a cue. Our findings and those of Holtzman et al. (1981)
argue that they do. Similar issues arise at the level of the parietal lobe. Our results suggest that
attention vastly improves the efficiency of the damaged parietal lobe. Some neural system is
producing the enhanced efficiency due to a cue. It seems unlikely that the strong enhancement
is due to the damaged parietal lobe.
I t might be argued that attention to spatial information involves an entirely different attention system than is used for language information. I n this view, attention in the spatial domain
might be seen as an independent module. Our results argue against this view. When peripheral
cues are presented in tasks when the subject's attention is occupied by a central language task,
(i.e. counting backward by threes), the cues do not produce normal facilitation (G. R . J. Hockey
& M. I. Posner 1980, unpublished). This result shows that spatial attention draws upon at least
some of the internal systems that are involved in attention to language and thus argue against
interpreting spatial attention experiments as an entirely separate domain ofattention. We suggest
from this that attention itself is probably not a strictly parietal function, but is mediated by the
parietal system when it involves certain tasks like spatial orienting. Perhaps a close examination
of the ability of the subject to execute commands to shift attention in the visual field will provide
an indication of the system that serves as the source of the attentional effects that we have
observed.
I n any case, both the time-locking of spatial attention to cues and the ability to show effects
of lesions at various levels of the nervous system suggest that the method that we have been using
provides a tool useful in obtaining a more complete understanding of the internal mechanisms
underlying at least one important cognitive function.
This research was supported in part by a grant from the National Science Foundation, grant
no. BNS 79-23627, to the University of Oregon and in part by a grant from the Murdock
Foundation to the Neurological Sciences Center, Good Samaritan Hospital, Portland, Oregon.
The authors are grateful to Dr 0. S. M. Marin for his support and assistance in this research.
et al.)
REFERENCE
(Posner
S
Bashinski, H. S. & Bachrach, V. R. 1980 Enhancement of perceptual sensitivity as the result of selectively attending to spatial locations. Percept. Psychophys. 28, 241-248.
Chang, F. R. 1981 Distribution of attention within a single fixation in reading: studies of the perceptual span.
Ph.D. thesis, University of Oregon.
Goodale, M. A. 1980 Visuomotor deficits in patients with progressive supranuclear palsy. Paper delivered to the
International Neuropsychological Society, San Francisco.
Goldberg, M. E. & Wurtz, R. H. 1972 Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkeys. 11. Effect of attention
on neuronal responses. J . Neurophysiol. 35, 560-574.
Holtzman, J. D., Sidtis, J.J., Volpe, B. T., Wilson, D. H. & Gazzaniga, M. S. 1981 Dissociation of spatial
information for stimulus localization and control of attention. Brain, 104, 861-872.
Mountcastle, V. B. 1978 Brain mechanisms for directed attention. J1 R. Soc. Med. 71, 14-27.
Pinker, S. 1980 Mental imagery and the third dimension. J . exp. Psychol.: gen. 109, 354-371.
Posner, M. I. 1978 Chronometric explorations of mind: the third Paul M. Fitts lectures. Hillside, N . J . : Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Posner, M. I. 1980 Orienting of attention. Q. J1 exp. Psychol. 32, 3-25.
Posner, M. I. & Cohen, Y. 1 9 8 0 ~Attention and the control of movements. In Tutorials in motor behavior (ed. G. E.
Stelmach & J. Requin), pp. 243-258. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Posner, M. I. & Cohen, Y. 1980b Consequences of visual orienting. Paper presented to the Psychonomics Society,
November 1980.
M. I. POSNER, Y. C O H E N A N D R. D. R A F A L
Posner, M. I. & Rothbart, M. 1981 The development of attentional mechanisms. In Nebraska Synzposium on
Motizlation (td. J . H . Flowers), pp. 1-52. Lincoln, Nebraska: University of Nebraska Press.
Rafal, R. D. & Grimm, R. J. 1981 Progressive supranuclear palsy: functional analysis of the response to
methysergide and anti-Parkinson agents. Neurology. 31, 1507-1518.
Rafal, R. D., Posner, M. I . & Walker, J. A. 1981 Spatial orienting and visually guided behavior: clinical and
experimental observations in patients with progressive supranuclear palsy. Paper presented to the International Neuropsychology Society, Atlanta.
Remington, R. W. 1980 Attention and saccadic eye movements. J. exp. Psychol. :hum. Percept. Perform. 6, 726-744.
Robinson, D. L., Goldberg, h?. E. & Stanton, G. B. 1980 Parietal association cortex in the primate: sensory
mechanisms and behavioral modulations. J . Neurophys. 41, 910-932.
Shulman, G. L. 1979 Spatial determinants of attention allocation. Ph.D. thesis, University of Oregon.
Shulman, G. L., Remington, R. W. & hlcLean, J. P. 1979 &loving attention through visual space. J. exp.
Psychol. : hum. Percept. Perform. 5, 522-526.
Treisman, A. & Gelade, G. 1980 A feature integration theory of attention. Cogn. Psychol. 12, 97-136.
Walker, J. A,, Posner, M. I. & Rafal, R. D. 1982 Separation of effects on cognition versus motor performance
due to the basal ganglia dysfunction of Parkinson patients. Presented at International Neuropsychological
Society, Pittsburg.
Weinstein, E. A. & Friedland, R. P. 1977 Htmi-inattention and hemispheric specialization. New York: Raven Press.
Weiskrantz, L. 1980 Varieties of residual experience. Q. J l exp. Psychol. 32, 365-386.
Wurtz, R. H., Goldberg, M. E. & Robinson, E. L. 1980 Behavioral modulation of visual responses in the monkey:
stimulus selection for attention and movement. Prog. Psychobiol. physiol. Psychol. 9, 43-83.