Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Regular Patterns in Cerebellar Purkinje Cell Simple Spike Trains

2007, PLoS ONE

Background. Cerebellar Purkinje cells (PC) in vivo are commonly reported to generate irregular spike trains, documented by high coefficients of variation of interspike-intervals (ISI). In strong contrast, they fire very regularly in the in vitro slice preparation. We studied the nature of this difference in firing properties by focusing on short-term variability and its dependence on behavioral state. Methodology/Principal Findings. Using an analysis based on CV 2 values, we could isolate precise regular spiking patterns, lasting up to hundreds of milliseconds, in PC simple spike trains recorded in both anesthetized and awake rodents. Regular spike patterns, defined by low variability of successive ISIs, comprised over half of the spikes, showed a wide range of mean ISIs, and were affected by behavioral state and tactile stimulation. Interestingly, regular patterns often coincided in nearby Purkinje cells without precise synchronization of individual spikes. Regular patterns exclusively appeared during the up state of the PC membrane potential, while single ISIs occurred both during up and down states. Possible functional consequences of regular spike patterns were investigated by modeling the synaptic conductance in neurons of the deep cerebellar nuclei (DCN). Simulations showed that these regular patterns caused epochs of relatively constant synaptic conductance in DCN neurons. Conclusions/Significance. Our findings indicate that the apparent irregularity in cerebellar PC simple spike trains in vivo is most likely caused by mixing of different regular spike patterns, separated by single long intervals, over time. We propose that PCs may signal information, at least in part, in regular spike patterns to downstream DCN neurons.

Regular Patterns in Cerebellar Purkinje Cell Simple Spike Trains Soon-Lim Shin1, Freek E. Hoebeek2, Martijn Schonewille2, Chris I. De Zeeuw2, Ad Aertsen3, Erik De Schutter1,4* 1 Theoretical Neurobiology, University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium, 2 Department of Neuroscience, Erasmus MC, Rotterdam, The Netherlands, 3 Neurobiology and Biophysics, Faculty of Biology and Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Albert-Ludwigs-University, Freiburg, Germany, 4 Computational Neuroscience Unit, Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology, Okinawa, Japan Background. Cerebellar Purkinje cells (PC) in vivo are commonly reported to generate irregular spike trains, documented by high coefficients of variation of interspike-intervals (ISI). In strong contrast, they fire very regularly in the in vitro slice preparation. We studied the nature of this difference in firing properties by focusing on short-term variability and its dependence on behavioral state. Methodology/Principal Findings. Using an analysis based on CV2 values, we could isolate precise regular spiking patterns, lasting up to hundreds of milliseconds, in PC simple spike trains recorded in both anesthetized and awake rodents. Regular spike patterns, defined by low variability of successive ISIs, comprised over half of the spikes, showed a wide range of mean ISIs, and were affected by behavioral state and tactile stimulation. Interestingly, regular patterns often coincided in nearby Purkinje cells without precise synchronization of individual spikes. Regular patterns exclusively appeared during the up state of the PC membrane potential, while single ISIs occurred both during up and down states. Possible functional consequences of regular spike patterns were investigated by modeling the synaptic conductance in neurons of the deep cerebellar nuclei (DCN). Simulations showed that these regular patterns caused epochs of relatively constant synaptic conductance in DCN neurons. Conclusions/Significance. Our findings indicate that the apparent irregularity in cerebellar PC simple spike trains in vivo is most likely caused by mixing of different regular spike patterns, separated by single long intervals, over time. We propose that PCs may signal information, at least in part, in regular spike patterns to downstream DCN neurons. Citation: Shin S-L, Hoebeek FE, Schonewille M, De Zeeuw CI, Aertsen A, et al (2007) Regular Patterns in Cerebellar Purkinje Cell Simple Spike Trains. PLoS ONE 2(5): e485. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000485 Rompun, Bayer, Leverkusen, Germany) in normal saline (0.9% NaCl, Baxter, Lessine, Belgium) by intraperitoneal injection. A craniotomy exposing Crus I and II of the left cerebellar hemisphere was performed [16]. Supplemental doses (one-third initial dose) were given intramuscularly to maintain deep anesthesia as evidenced by the lack of a pinch withdrawal reflex and/or lack of whisking. Forty eight single unit recordings were made in the cerebellar cortex with tungsten microelectrodes (impedance ,10 MOhm, FHC, Bowdoinham, ME). Signals were filtered and amplified (bandpass = 0.5–9 kHz; gain = 5,000– 10,000) using a multichannel neuronal acquisition processor (Plexon Inc., Austin, TX) and collected spike trains were analyzed off-line using NEX (Plexon Inc.). After recordings of spontaneous activity, 12 stimulus-evoked responses were recorded in 10 rats. Perioral receptive fields were explored as reported elsewhere [16]. The punctate stimulus was applied at a rate of 0.5 Hz. In a separate series of experiments reported in more detail INTRODUCTION The cerebellum is crucial for the precise temporal control of motor related tasks [1] and conditioned behaviors [2]. Yet, it is not clear how the cerebellum may signal precise timing at the cellular level. Prior studies of spike time coding in the cerebellum have focused on the discharge of Purkinje cells (PCs), which form the sole output of cerebellar cortex. However, thus far these studies only considered mean firing rates of the simple spikes (SS) [3,4] or complex spikes (CS) [5,6]. Little attention has been paid to their fine-temporal structure, even though spike timing may encode additional information in other systems [7–12]. In fact, for two different strains of ataxic mice with mutations of voltage-gated calcium channels expressed in PCs it was recently reported that PCs show increased irregularity of firing [13,14]. A common measure to characterize the temporal structure of spike trains is the coefficient of variation (CV) of the interspike intervals (ISIs). The CV of SS firing of PCs recorded in vivo is reported to be quite high [15,16]: close to or even higher than 1, the CV of a Poisson process. Conversely, PCs in the in vitro slice preparation fire very regularly [17,18]. To test whether this difference in firing properties is as large as is commonly assumed and to investigate its possible functional importance, we analyzed the fine-temporal structure of SS trains in different preparations and behavioral states in more detail, focusing on the short-term variability. Academic Editor: Miguel Nicolelis, Duke University, United States of America Received March 11, 2007; Accepted May 2, 2007; Published May 30, 2007 Copyright: ß 2007 Shin et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Funding: This study was funded by the European Commission, in Belgium by IUAP, GSKE, FWO, in the Netherlands by NOW, NeuroBsik, Prinses Beatrix Fonds, and in Germany by the BMBF. These organisations had not influence on the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data; or writing of the paper; and on the decision to submit it for publication to PloS One. MATERIALS AND METHODS Recordings Sprague-Dawley rats (n = 26, 300–500 g, Iffa Credo, Brussels, Belgium) were anesthetized with a mixture of ketamine HCl (75 mg/kg; Ketalar, Parke-Davis, Warner Lambert Manufacturing, Dublin, Ireland) and xylazine HCl (3.9 mg/kg; Rats PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist. * To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected] 1 May 2007 | Issue 5 | e485 Regular Patterns in Cerebellum 238 mV, 238 mV, and 240 mV in cell 1, 2, and 3 respectively, the spike was sorted as a CS. Mean firing rate of SS (CS) was 12.865.2 Hz (1.160.4 Hz). The threshold to distinguish UP from DOWN states was set to 255 mV. Regular patterns and single intervals were isolated as in the extracellular recordings. Data are represented as mean6s.e.m., unless otherwise stated. All p-values refer to Student’s paired or unpaired t-test, unless otherwise specified. in [19], 8 transverse pairs of nearby PCs were recorded using similar procedures. Electric lesions were made after recordings to measure the distance between pairs and the distance between the centers of lesions was measured. In the context of this paper, the data from all these experiments were re-analyzed. All experimental methods were approved by the University of Antwerp and conformed to European Union guidelines. Mice Extracellular activity was recorded in the cerebellar flocculus and paramedian lobule using glass micropipettes filled with 2 M NaCl (tip diameter: 2–5 mm; impedance: 2.5 MV at 1 kHz) in either restrained awake or anesthetized (with mixture of ketamine (50 mg/kg) and xylazine (10 mg/kg)) C57BL/6 mice. The electrode tip was positioned on the cerebellar surface under visual guidance (Olympus VS-IV; Olympus Optical, Tokyo, Japan) using a micromanipulator (David Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA) and moved downward by a hydraulic microdrive (Trent Wells) equipped with a stepping motor (TL Elektronik SMS 87). The electrode signal was amplified and filtered (bandwidth 10–6000 Hz; Dagan 2400; Dagan, Minneapolis, MN) and sampled at 12.5 kHz (CED 1401plus, Spike2, Cambridge, UK). Single-unit PCs were identified on-line by the presence of a brief pause in simple spikes after the complex spike. In the off-line analysis, SSs and CSs were detected and discriminated using custom-made software implemented in Matlab (Mathworks, Natick, MA). All experimental methods were approved by Erasmus MC in Rotterdam, and conformed to European Union guidelines. Stochastic Modeling of Poisson processes Both spontaneous and evoked spike trains were modeled using an inhomogeneous Poisson process (see also [25]) with 2 ms of dead time (which is equal to the detection window used in the electrophysiological recordings). The probability density of the process can be described as Pn(t)~rn e{rn t H(t{2), where t denotes the time elapsed since the last spike, rn is the mean firing 5 where n = 3, 4, …, rate at the n-th ISI, estimated by rn~ P nz2 ISI i i~n{2 number of spikes-2, H (t22) is Heaviside function standing for 1 only when t is 2 ms or larger. The mean firing rates of realized spike trains (51.762.4 Hz, N = 92) were statistically similar to those of recorded SS trains (51.762.4 Hz, N = 92, p.0.6). For evoked spike trains, firing rate was estimated from the rate distribution around stimulation time (bin size: 1 ms, lag = 1 s). Based on these estimates, model spike times were created trial by trial. Then, a final spike train was constructed by concatenating spike times in consecutive trials. Simulations were performed using Matlab. Spike timing analysis Synaptic Conductance Modeling Data analysis of extracellular recordings Recordings were 83 to 1202 sec long and comprised 1,328 to 62,371 spikes. Analysis was carried out using Matlab and Excel (Microsoft). Short-term regularity was measured with CV2 = 2|ISIn+12ISIn|/ (ISIn+1+ISIn) [20]. The number of regular patterns was measured using a threshold value for CV2 ranging from 0 to 2 with an increment of 0.02. In each PC the numbers were normalized by the maximum number of patterns to avoid an influence of the difference between firing rates. The strength of synchronization of regular patterns was measured using a standard score, the Z score of the amplitude of the central peak of the cross-correlogram [21]: Z = (Nc2Ne)/(SDe), with Nc the number of spikes in the central peak (bin = 5 ms), Ne the mean number of spikes in a 2 sec window between 21 and 1 sec, and SDe its standard deviation. To determine whether the observed synchronization (Z score of 3 or higher) reflected spike to spike precise synchronization or rather co-modulation of firing rates [22,23], simulated spike trains were generated by randomly shuffling the ISIs within blocks of 5 consecutive ISIs and the correlation analysis repeated for the shuffled spike trains. The correlation between CSs and patterns were analyzed in 32 PCs where CSs were well isolated. If the duration from the ends of patterns to CSs was longer than 1.2 times the pattern mean ISI, it was considered longer than the pattern mean ISI. Data analysis of whole-cell clamp recordings Three membrane potential traces recorded from 3 different anesthetized rats were analyzed to investigate the relation between spike patterns and the membrane potential [24]. The sampling frequencies of the original recordings (50, 50, and 20 kHz) were sampled down to 10 kHz without loosing discriminative power. Spikes were sorted by setting thresholds at 238 mV, 242 mV, and 232 mV in cell1, cell2 and cell3 respectively. Spikes were further sorted as either CS or SS by checking the mean membrane potential between 2 and 4 ms after a spike. If the mean membrane potential was higher than PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org The dynamics of multiple pulse depression of the synapse between PCs and DCN neurons were described previously [26,27]. The data reported in these papers are quantitatively different, even though they report the same phenomena, probably due to different experimental conditions (e.g. recording temperature). Our phenomenological model is based on the Pedroarena and Schwarz study [26], because it measured multiple pulse depression at more frequencies over a large range (from 1 to 200 Hz). The depression is assumed to be caused by changes of the presynaptic release probability (R). We fitted a deterministic model for R to the multiple pulse depression data. This deterministic approach is justified because the large number of release sites from which transmitter can reach all receptors [28] makes synaptic failures unlikely. We fitted equations for the steady state level of release probability Rss and the time constant of depression t to the data: Rss (r)~0:08z0:60 e{2:84 r z0:32 e{0:02 r t (r)~2z2500 e{0:274 r z100 e{0:022 r where r is the instantaneous firing rate computed as the inverse of the last interspike interval. Rss and t are updated at the time of occurrence of each spike n and Rn is then computed as: Rn ~Rn 1 z( Rss {Rn 1 )(1{ e{ ISI n t ) with Rn21 is the release probability computed at the previous spike time, ISIn is the interspike interval between the current spike and the previous spike. See Figure 1 for the accuracy of the fit of our model to the experimental data. Synaptic conductance (Gsyn) during spiking was modeled by a double exponential function multiplied by Rn and calculated over 2 May 2007 | Issue 5 | e485 Regular Patterns in Cerebellum Figure 1. Simulation of PC to DCN synaptic conductance. (A) Saturating level of release probability (Rss) taken from Pedroarena and Schwarz (2003) could be modeled with a double exponential function (red line, see Materials and Methods for details). (B) Simulated synaptic conductance profiles in response to 10, 30 and 100 Hz PC firing, respectively. These results should be compared to Figure 7A of Pedroarena and Schwarz (2003). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000485.g001 200 ms with a resolution of 0.1 ms following each spike: Gsyn (t)~ Gpre zA Gmax {t {t Rn( e t1 { e t2 ) t1 { t2 Here, Gpre is the synaptic conductance caused by previous spikes, A = 15.5 is a constant to scale the maximum conductance to the experimental value of Gmax (11.7 nS), t1 is 12 ms, and t2 is 1.2 ms. Gmax, t1, and t2, were chosen to fit the multiple pulse depression traces shown in Figure 7 of Pedroarena et al [26]. The multiple pulse depression following 10, 30 and 100 Hz stimulation of the PC is shown in Figure 1. Simple spike trains contain precise regular spiking patterns Figure 2. Regular patterns in cerebellar Purkinje cell simple spike trains. (A) Raster plot of PC SS in an anesthetized rat (AnR). (B) CV2 distributions of SS trains recorded from anesthetized mice (AnM, left), awake mice (AwM, middle, blue: neurons in cerebral motor cortex), and mean of 92 CV2 distributions (Pooled, right) which were significantly different from those of inhomogeneous Poisson processes with similarly modulated firing rates (p,0.05, x2 test; *: p,0.001, x2 goodness of fit residual test; red line: CV2 = 0.2). Insets and right panel: mean6s.e.m. (black: PC, green: inhomogeneous Poisson process) (C) Extracting regular spiking patterns by setting CV2 threshold at 0.2 (white dotted lines). White dashes: CV2 values calculated from the two surrounding ISIs, red: first ISI of regular patterns, pink: successive ISIs in regular patterns, dark blue: ISIs not belonging to a regular pattern). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000485.g002 We analyzed spontaneous PC activity in 3 data sets: recordings from the cerebellar hemisphere of anesthetized rats (AnR, n = 48) and from the flocculus or paramedian lobule of anesthetized (AnM, n = 21) and awake (AwM, n = 37) mice. Firing rates were similar for all data sets (Table 1). As expected, CVs of the spike trains were high: 3.9360.49 (AnR), 1.7460.47 (AnM) and 1.3960.38 (AwM), consistent with previous reports [6,16] and suggestive of highly irregular firing in vivo. Nevertheless, careful visual inspection of the individual spike trains revealed clear patterns of regular firing (Figure 2A). To characterize these patterns we used a short range measure which compares two adjacent ISIs, i.e. the CV2 (cf. Materials and Methods; [20]). Surprisingly, we found that in all data sets the mean CV2 was low (AnR: 0.5160.03, AnM: 0.3060.02, AwM: 0.3960.02), suggestive of much more regular firing at short time scales. In fact, most PCs showed a skewed CV2 distribution, with a high proportion of low CV2 values (Figure 2B), indicating the presence of regularity in spiking patterns. This was in clear contrast to spontaneous spiking of neocortical neurons, which showed uniform CV2 distributions as previously reported [20] (Figure 2B, blue) and which are similar to realizations of inhomogeneous Poisson processes (insets in Figure 2B, green). We studied the properties of regular spiking patterns in PCs whose CV2 distribution was significantly different from rate modulated Poisson (AnR: n = 38, AnM: n = 21, AwM: n = 33, p,0.05, x2 test) in more detail. Their pooled CV2 distribution showed significantly more CV2 values of 0.2 or lower (p,0.001, x2 ................................. RESULTS Table 1. Summary of spontaneous simple spike firing properties of all PCs reported in this study. .................................................................................................................................................. Anesthetized rats Number of PCs Mean firing rate %Long ISI (ISI.1 s) CV Mean CV2 Maximum pattern size 48 45.564.1 0.4160.11 3.9360.49 0.5160.03 28.964.4 Awake mice 37 51.062.7 (p.0.2) 0.0160.00 (p,0.001) 1.3960.38 (p,10 Anesthetized mice 21 49.863.6 (p.0.4, p*.0.7) 0.0260.01 (p,0.001, p*.0.1) 24 1.7460.47 (p,0.05, p*.0.4) ) 0.3960.02 (p,0.001) 13.060.6 (p,0.001) 0.3060.02 (p,1026, p*,0.05) 24.962.8 (p.0.4, p*,0.001) p: comparison to anesthetized rats, p*: comparison to awake mice, Student t test. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000485.t001 PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 3 May 2007 | Issue 5 | e485 Regular Patterns in Cerebellum mean ISI. Examples of the distribution of these two parameters are shown in Figure 4B. Observe that short patterns occurred with a wide range of mean ISIs, whereas long patterns contained only short ISIs (insets), though not the shortest. The wide range of pattern mean ISIs and the fact that the fraction of pattern spikes was only weakly (rats: linear correlation R2 = 0.371 compared to a R2 = 0.666 for 92 inhomogeneous Poisson processes) or not (mice: R2,0.1) dependent on the mean firing rates of PCs make it unlikely that regular patterns were caused by refractoriness. Pattern sizes showed a wide distribution. On average, 72% of patterns comprised only 2–3 ISIs, but many patterns were much longer (Figure 4B and 4C), lasting 45.063.5, 76.566.3, and 52.563.1 ms for AnR, AnM, and AwM, respectively. The size of patterns depended on the CV2 threshold used, but this did not affect the pattern mean ISI much (data not shown). Interestingly, we found a significant difference in the proportion of long patterns between anesthetized and awake rodents. In anesthetized rodents, 4.161.4% (AnR) and 3.560.8% (AnM) of patterns contained more than 10 ISIs, while in awake rodents (AwM) only 0.460.1% did (p,0.01), with maximum pattern sizes of 182, 61 and 21 ISIs, respectively (Figure 4C). This significant difference between pattern sizes of awake vs. anesthetized rodents indicates that regular patterns may be influenced by the behavioral state of the animal. Indeed, if regular spiking patterns were a specific signal by which PCs transmit information, one would predict faster changes in this signal, i.e. shorter patterns associated with a wider range of pattern mean ISIs, in awake, active animals than in anesthetized ones. Figure 3. Effect of CV2 threshold on patterns. (A) Mean (6 s.e.m.) of normalized number of patterns in spike trains classified with different values of the CV2 threshold, ranging from 0 to 0.5, in 92 PCs (filled circles) and in simulated spike trains from Poisson processes with similar firing rate profiles as in the PCs (open circles). Arrow: maximum number of patterns, *: range where there was no statistical difference (p.0.05). Inset: same distribution but for all possible thresholds. (B) Raster plots with indication of the spike timings belonging to patterns (red dotted lines: start of patterns, red solid lines: following spikes in each pattern) and singles (blue). Black dots: difference in classified patterns when threshold was 0.2 (upper trace) and 0.24 (lower trace). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000485.g003 Simulation of the effects of regular spiking patterns on target neurons goodness of fit residual test) than the corresponding Poisson processes did (Figure 2B, rightmost panel). To isolate the regular spiking patterns in individual spike trains, we applied a threshold of 0.2 on the measured CV2 values as illustrated in Figure 2C. Whenever the CV2 value was below or equal to threshold (white dotted line), the associated two ISIs were considered part of a regular pattern (pink). If the next ISI also had a CV2 value below or equal to threshold, it was included into the pattern (pink); if not, this next ISI was either single (i.e. not belonging to a pattern; blue) or the start of a new pattern (provided the next CV2 value was again below or equal to threshold; red). With this procedure, 57% (AnR), 67% (AnM) and 54% (AwM) of ISIs belonged to regular patterns. To verify the effect of the statistically defined CV2 threshold of 0.2, we compared the number of patterns extracted using different thresholds (Figure 3). Thresholds in the range 0.18–0.24 generated statistically similar number of patterns as a threshold of 0.22, which generated the maximum number of patterns (n = 92, p.0.1). The mean ISIs of patterns were not uniformly distributed. Most of the pattern ISIs were relatively short, so that the peaks of the overall ISI distributions mostly consisted of regular patterns, while their tails comprised only single ISIs. As a result, the 90 percentile of ISI for patterns was significantly shorter than that of singles (Figure 4A). PCs inhibit neurons in the downstream DCN; any information transmitted by regular PC spike patterns will be decoded at that level. PC synapses onto DCN neurons show fast synaptic depression [26,27], a property that is known to endow synapses with low-pass filtering properties [29]. We developed a phenomenological model to reproduce the previously reported frequencydependent depression of this synapse [26] (cf. Materials and Methods), allowing us to predict the effects of regular PC spike patterns on synaptic conductance (Gsyn) in DCN neurons. Specifically, we used this model to compare the Gsyn evoked by recorded SS trains with that of simulated spike trains generated by inhomogeneous Poisson processes of the same modulated firing rates. Such Poisson processes have far fewer regular patterns: only 20% of ISIs belonged to patterns, 80% of which were of size 2 (cf. Fig. 2B). A representative example of conductance traces (Figure 5A) demonstrates that the long regular patterns in SS trains induced epochs with little fluctuation of Gsyn, while Poisson spike trains generated much more variable Gsyn (Figure 5A, green). In almost all cases, the distribution of Gsyn of Poisson spike trains was significantly different from that of real SS trains (Figure 5B; Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, p,0.05, bin 0.01 s, AnR 35/38 cells; AnM 33/33; AwM 20/21). Thus, the distribution of Gsyn of PCs was mostly confined to a narrow range of values as is also evident from its CV, which was significantly lower for PCs (0.5360.03, 0.4760.03 and 0.4960.03 for AnR, AnM and AwM, respectively) than for simulated spike trains from Poisson processes (0.6660.05, 0.7560.04, and 0.7160.03, p,0.04). Characteristics of regular spiking patterns change with behavioral state Spikes of regular patterns are correlated, but not precisely synchronized Regular patterns can be characterized by two parameters: pattern size, defined as the number of ISIs in the pattern, and pattern In rats, each DCN neuron receives inhibition from 100 [30] up to 1000 [31] PCs. Anatomically, though, it is not clear whether all PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 4 May 2007 | Issue 5 | e485 Regular Patterns in Cerebellum Figure 4. Characteristics of regular spike patterns. (A) ISI distribution of overall ISIs (black), patterns (red) and singles (blue) from a representative sample PC spike train of AnR (left), AnM (middle) and AwM (right). Insets: magnified plot of indicated area (lower) and 90 P (90 percentile, upper) of each population, *: p,0.01, Student t test. (B) The relation between pattern mean ISI and pattern size in AnR (left, cyan), AnM (middle, magenta) and AwM (right, yellow). Insets: maximum pattern mean ISI (90 percentile) of different pattern sizes. *: p,0.001, Wilcoxon signed rank test. (C) Percentage ISIs belonging to patterns (upper, *: p,0.001, Student t test), Average maximum pattern size (middle, *: p,0.001, Student t test), and Pattern size distribution (lower, p,0.05, x2 test). Cyan: AnR, magenta: AnM, yellow: AwM. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000485.g004 Figure 5. Simulated synaptic conductance in PC to DCN synapse caused by spontaneous PC spiking. (A) A representative example of the simulated synaptic conductance (Gsyn) induced by PC (black) of AnR (upper panel), AnM (middle panel) and AwM (lower panel), and by corresponding realizations of an inhomogeneous Poisson process (green). Rasters: spikes belonging to patterns (black and green dotted lines: start of patterns, black and green solid lines: following spikes in patterns, blue lines: singles), numbers: number of all spikes in the 500 ms window. (B) Distribution of Gsyn values for PCs (black) compared to Poisson processes (green). Bin = 0.2 nS. Red bar: bins where PCs contained significantly more Gsyn values. p,0.05. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000485.g005 PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 5 May 2007 | Issue 5 | e485 Regular Patterns in Cerebellum synchronized, overall pattern spikes were not precisely synchronized but tended to co-occur in a loose manner because of firing rate co-modulation. Despite our lack of knowledge about detailed convergence patterns of PCs onto DCN neurons, convergence is more likely for adjacent pairs, where we observed coincident patterns, than for distant pairs which did not show synchronization. In the case of coincident converging patterns, the lack of spike synchronization caused by their different spike frequencies will further reduce the variability of their combined Gsyn [32]. Otherwise, the averaged Gsyn of perfectly synchronized PCs would have the same variability as that caused by single PCs. A similar reduction of variability also occurs in completely irregular spike trains generated by Poisson processes [32], but only when there are many more active convergent inputs. Tactile stimulation increases regularity of spiking Next, the effect of sensory stimulation on regular patterns in the SS response was investigated. To this end, we analyzed responses to tactile stimulation in AnR (Figure 7) (n = 12). Typically, PCs responded after a short delay with a significant increase in SS firing rate in a 200 ms window, from 53.866.2 Hz to 74.267.3 Hz (p,0.003, Wilcoxon signed ranks test), as reported elsewhere [33]. In the same window, we also found a significantly increased proportion of ISIs belonging to regular patterns, from 49.364.5% to 62.565.1% (p,0.005, Wilcoxon signed ranks test). Spike trains always become more regular at high firing rates because spikes cannot fire arbitrarily close together, due to the refractory period. Indeed, simulated Poisson processes with refractory period that show similar rate changes (p.0.2; before: 53.366.3 Hz, after: 75.067.5 Hz; Figure 7E upper panel) as the experimental data revealed a slight but statistically significant increase of the fraction of ISIs belonging to regular patterns (p,.005; before: 23.160.7%, after: 27.160.8%; Figure 7E lower panel). However, this increase was proportionally (1862%) much smaller than the increase in PCs (2963%) (p,0.03, Wilcoxon signed ranks test). We conclude that the increase in patterns in PCs was larger than expected from only the firing rate increase. Regular patterns following stimulation were also faster and lasted longer than before stimulation (Figure 7F). To estimate the effect of this change of SS patterns on downstream DCN neurons, we again computed the predicted Gsyn and compared these with the results obtained from spike trains realized from inhomogeneous Poisson processes (Figure 7C). The real SS train induced a steady current of about 4.5 nS, while the Poisson process produced a highly variable Gsyn, despite the similar modulations of firing rates. The CV of Gsyn induced by the real SS train dropped significantly (p,0.001) during a period of 116.7619.0 ms after stimulus onset compared to the effect of Poisson spike trains (Figure 7D). This indicates that tactile stimulation further reduced the variability of Gsyn in DCN neurons by an increased regularity of PC spike timing. Figure 6. Coincident patterns in nearby PC pairs in AnR. (A) Eight cross-correlograms of timings of spikes belonging to regular patterns extracted from recordings of nearby PC pairs, with each pair colored differently. Insets: cross-correlograms of the shuffled spike trains of two pairs (black) superimposed on original cross-correlogram of patterns (blue and gray: pairs showing strongest and weakest synchronization respectively). (B) The relation of pattern mean ISIs in 4 pairs in which pattern starts coincided significantly (inset: cross-correlograms of the first spikes of regular patterns in the 4 pairs). Red dotted line: diagonal. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000485.g006 converging inputs are active at the same time. This convergence raises the question whether regular patterns in the afferent PCs coincide in time, causing periodic ripples in Gsyn during their occurrence, or whether they are asynchronous, rendering Gsyn more constant. We studied the correlation in time of regular patterns in 8 simultaneously recorded transverse pairs of PCs in AnR, separated by 69.869.4 mm (range: 50–100 mm). We found that the spikes belonging to patterns revealed central peaks in the cross-correlogram. These central peaks reflected significant synchronization as their Z scores were higher than 3 (Figure 6A, z = 5.060.4), but they were quite broad (full width at half peak (HW) = 7068.6 ms). No synchronization was observed in pairs of PCs on the same parallel fiber beam separated by more than 0.5 mm (n = 20, data not shown). We investigated several mechanisms that could explain the broad width of the central peaks. There was no significantly relation between HW and the mean duration of patterns (R2,0.0001). Broad peaks in crosscorrelograms are often caused by firing rate co-modulation [23], implying that patterns would coincide because they occur more often during increased firing rates (Figure 4A). As properly shuffled spike trains (cf. Materials and Methods) overlapped the broad peaks largely (Figure 6A, insets), the central peaks observed can indeed be largely explained by firing rate co-modulation. In addition, we found in four of the pairs a significant, and much more precise, correlation of the start of patterns (z = 5.660.9, HW = 16.362.4 ms, Figure 6B inset). But although patterns in these four pairs started together, their mean ISIs were independent of each other (R2 = 0.1760.03, Figure 6B). We conclude that while for a fraction of patterns the start was precisely PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org Regular patterns and singles in relation to the PC membrane potential It has been shown that the membrane potential of PCs in anesthetized animals can be bistable, showing up and down-states [24]. Although PCs in awake behaving animals probably operate predominantly in the up-state [34] and regular patterns in our data tended to last much shorter than the reported duration of upstates in the anesthetized preparation [24], the occurrence of patterns might in principle be related to the state of the membrane potential. We therefore applied our analysis method to whole-cell 6 May 2007 | Issue 5 | e485 Regular Patterns in Cerebellum Figure 7. A representative example of regular patterns in tactile stimulus evoked PC SS responses. (A) Peri-event raster plot of patterns (red) and singles (blue) during tactile stimulation in AnR. (B) Mean rate (6 s.e.m.) of overall spikes (black), realization of Poisson process (green), pattern spikes (red) and singles (blue). Bin = 20 ms. (C) Simulated Gsyn for the trial indicated by arrow in (A) (bin = 1 ms). (D) CV (SD/Mean) of simulated Gsyn (*: p,0.001, Student t test, bin 20 ms). Black dotted line: stimulation time. (E) Mean firing rate (upper panel) and percent ISIs belonging to regular patterns (lower panel) in 200 ms before and after stimulation (upper panel) of simulated spike trains from inhomogeneous Poisson process (green) and from recorded PCs (black). *: p,0.005, Wilcoxon signed ranks test. (F) Pattern mean ISI distribution before (dotted line) and after (solid line) tactile stimulation. Inset: Pattern size distribution before (open) and after (filled) stimulation. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000485.g007 clamp recordings from PCs of anesthetized rats in vivo (data obtained from Loewenstein et al., 2005; Figure 8). As expected, patterns occurred only during up-states (Figure 8, red spikes), but a single up-state typically consisted of several patterns (4.861.1 for the recording shown in Figure 8, 3.860.3 for all recordings). Singles could occur during both up (92.163.7%; Figure 8, filled dots) or down (7.963.7%, Figure 8, open dots) states. Thus, the classification of SSs developed in this study allows for a subcategorization of spikes occurring during the up-state which may be relevant at short time scales. CSs may toggle transitions between up and down-states [24]. This is also the case for start of the two up-states shown in Figure 8. We found that, except for patterns occurring at state transitions, the start or end of patterns was not related to CS firing. This is confirmed by the much higher frequency of starts of patterns (7.4264.30 Hz, AnR, n = 32) than of CSs (0.7260.05 Hz). Figure 8. Regular patterns and singles related to the membrane potential (MP). Dendritic patch-clamp recording of PC in anesthetized rat (data from Loewenstein et al. 2005). Voltage trace: large spikes are complex spikes, small ones are simple spikes. Dotted black line: threshold to define up and down-states (MP = 255 mV). Raster plot at top: simple spikes were sorted as either pattern spikes (dotted red lines: start of patterns, solid red lines: following spikes in each pattern) or single spikes (blue lines). All patterns were during up-state, but singles occurred both during up (filled circles) and down (open circles) states. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000485.g008 DISCUSSION Taken together, our main findings indicate that (1) interesting finetemporal properties of neuronal responses may be uncovered by PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 7 May 2007 | Issue 5 | e485 Regular Patterns in Cerebellum analyzing regular pattern structure on a single trial basis; (2) PC simple spike trains contain distinctly more spike timing regularities than hitherto known; (3) the high CV in in vivo recordings is most likely caused by mixing of different regular spiking patterns, separated by single, typically longer, ISIs; (4) the onset of patterns can be synchronized in nearby PCs, but their member spikes are not synchronized; (5) most regular patterns are not influenced by complex spikes; (6) regular pattern properties change with behavioral state and tactile stimulation; and (7) regular patterns may cause epochs of close to constant synaptic conductance in downstream DCN neurons. Our extracellular recordings do not provide conclusive evidence on the mechanisms causing regular patterns. However, as PCs fire highly regularly in slice preparations in which their synaptic inputs are blocked [17,18] and since they show increased irregularity following mutations of their voltage gated Ca2+ channels [13], the endogenous properties of PCs are likely to contribute to their regularity of firing. However, the current observation that most patterns occur within the up-state, combined with the finding that PCs in awake behaving animals probably operate predominantly in the up-state [34] suggests that additional mechanisms such as short-term and long-term synaptic processes probably also play a role in controlling the start and end of a regular pattern, as well as its mean ISI. As CSs have little effect on patterns, it is most likely that parallel fiber inputs combined with molecular layer inhibition control the pattern properties. Furthermore, synaptic plasticity can adapt the effect of both the excitatory parallel fiber inputs and the inhibitory input from the basket cells and stellate cells on the SS patterns [35,36]. Such mechanisms could explain why the onset of patterns was synchronized in only a subset of nearby PCs, and why even in those cases the pattern mean ISIs were different. The regular patterns discovered in this study comprised a large part of the simple spike trains and were shown to be modulated by behavioral state and stimulation, suggesting that they may have functional significance. Our simulations of the effect of regular patterns on Gsyn in downstream DCN neurons indicate that they keep inhibitory conductance fairly constant. The interaction between regular patterns and Gsyn may provide an explanation of why these synapses depress so strongly [26,27] and forms the basis for our hypothesis on the function of regular patterns. It is generally assumed that cerebellar learning through induction of long-term depression at the parallel fiber to PC synapse leads to disinhibition of DCN neurons [35,37]. In addition, DCN neurons respond strongly to disinhibition because of their post-inhibitory rebound spike [38], which may form a powerful timing signal [2,39]. Correspondingly, the activity of DCN neurons in adult rodents consists of pauses, most likely caused by PC inhibition, mixed with transient periods of fast bursting [40]. The effectiveness of disinhibition to create a rebound spike depends on the synchronicity of the disinhibition, which we recently demonstrated to be significant among nearby PCs [19], and on the level of preceding inhibition. Because the inactivation of calcium channels expressed in the DCN neurons is strongly voltage dependent in the relevant potential range [41], these channels are very sensitive to even small changes in inhibitory input. Consequently, the level of inhibition preceding the rebound spike exerts a very strong effect on the amplitude of the rebound spike [42]. We hypothesize that regular patterns encode a specific level of inhibition in their firing rate and, as such, approximate a perfect firing rate code [43], which should be completely regular. When regular patterns from convergent PCs coincide, the summed inhibition will be relatively constant over the duration of the patterns and, consequently, keep the level of inactivation of calcium channels steady. Thereby, the firing rates of regular spike patterns in afferent PCs will control the amplitude of any rebound spike that follows in the next second. The occurrence of a rebound spike is evoked by synchronized pauses in the SS trains [19,44], which are mostly not part of the regular patterns as they belong to the tail of the ISI distribution. In conclusion, we propose that the regular patterns, which comprise the majority of spikes in PC SS trains, can control the amplitude of subsequent timing signals by modulating the amplitude of rebound spikes in downstream DCN neurons. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank Drs. Dana Cohen and Miguel Nicolelis for making the mice cortical recordings available and Drs Séverine Mahon and Mike Häusser for sharing the PC whole-cell clamp recordings. We thank Drs. Dieter Jaeger, Reinoud Maex, Martin Nawrot, Arnd Roth, Stefan Rotter and Cornelius Schwarz for comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. Author Contributions Conceived and designed the experiments: ED CD SS. Performed the experiments: FH MS SS. Analyzed the data: SS. Contributed reagents/ materials/analysis tools: ED FH MS AA SS. Wrote the paper: ED CD AA SS. Other: Obtained funding for the study: ED. REFERENCES 9. Vaadia E, Haalman I, Abeles M, Bergman H, Prut Y, et al. (1995) Dynamics of neuronal interactions in monkey cortex in relation to behavioural events. Nature 373: 515–518. 10. Riehle A, Grun S, Diesmann M, Aertsen A (1997) Spike synchronization and rate modulation differentially involved in motor cortical function. Science 278: 1950–1953. 11. Konishi M (2003) Coding of auditory space. Annu Rev Neurosci 26: 31–55. 12. Heil P (2004) First-spike latency of auditory neurons revisited. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14: 461–467. 13. Hoebeek FE, Stahl JS, van Alphen AM, Schonewille M, Luo C, et al. (2005) Increased noise level of Purkinje cell activities minimizes impact of their modulation during sensorimotor control. Neuron 45: 953–965. 14. Walter JT, Alvina K, Womack MD, Chevez C, Khodakhah K (2006) Decreases in the precision of Purkinje cell pacemaking cause cerebellar dysfunction and ataxia. Nat Neurosci 9: 389–397. 15. Goossens J, Daniel H, Rancillac A, van der Steen J, Oberdick J, et al. (2001) Expression of protein kinase C inhibitor blocks cerebellar long-term depression without affecting Purkinje cell excitability in alert mice. J NeuroSci 21: 5813. 16. Vos BP, Volny-Luraghi A, De Schutter E (1999) Cerebellar Golgi cells in the rat: receptive fields and timing of responses to facial stimulation. Eur J Neurosci 11: 2621–2634. 1. Ivry RB, Spencer RM (2004) The neural representation of time. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14: 225–232. 2. Koekkoek SK, Hulscher HC, Dortland BR, Hensbroek RA, Elgersma Y, et al. (2003) Cerebellar LTD and learning-dependent timing of conditioned eyelid responses. Science 301: 1736–1739. 3. Shidara M, Kawano K, Gomi H, Kawato M (1993) Inverse-dynamics model eye movement control by Purkinje cells in the cerebellum. Nature 365: 50–52. 4. Coltz JD, Johnson MT, Ebner TJ (1999) Cerebellar Purkinje cell simple spike discharge encodes movement velocity in primates during visuomotor arm tracking. J Neurosci 19: 1782–1803. 5. Kitazawa S, Kimura T, Yin PB (1998) Cerebellar complex spikes encode both destinations and errors in arm movements. Nature 392: 494–497. 6. Goossens HH, FEH, Van Alphen AM, Van Der Steen J, Stahl JS, et al. (2004) Simple spike and complex spike activity of floccular Purkinje cells during the optokinetic reflex in mice lacking cerebellar long-term depression. Eur J Neurosci 19: 687–697. 7. Rieke F, Warland D, de Ruyter van Steveninck RR, Bialek W (1997) Spikes. Exploring the neural code. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. 8. VanRullen R, Guyonneau R, Thorpe SJ (2005) Spike times make sense. Trends Neurosci 28: 1–4. PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 8 May 2007 | Issue 5 | e485 Regular Patterns in Cerebellum 31. Chan-Palay V (1977) Cerebellar Dentate Nucleus. New York: Springer-Verlag. 32. Softky WR, Koch C (1993) The highly irregular firing of cortical cells is inconsistent with temporal integration of random EPSPs. J Neurosci 13: 334–350. 33. Jaeger D, Bower JM (1994) Prolonged responses in rat cerebellar Purkinje cells following activation of the granule cell layer: an intracellular in vitro and in vivo investigation. Exp Brain Res 100: 200–214. 34. Schonewille M, Khosrovani S, Hoebeek FE, De Jeu MTG, Larsen IM, et al. (2006) Purkinje cells in awake behaving animals operate at the upstate membrane potential. Nat Neurosci 9: 459–461. 35. Ito M (2001) Cerebellar long-term depression: characterization, signal transduction, and functional roles. Physiol Rev 81: 1143–1195. 36. Jorntell H, Ekerot CF (2003) Receptive field plasticity profoundly alters the cutaneous parallel fiber synaptic input to cerebellar interneurons in vivo. J Neurosci 23: 9620–9631. 37. Ohyama T, Nores WL, Murphy M, Mauk MD (2003) What the cerebellum computes. Trends Neurosci 26: 222–227. 38. Aizenman CD, Linden DJ (1999) Regulation of the rebound depolarization and spontaneous firing patterns of deep nuclear neurons in slices of rat cerebellum. J Neurophysiol 82: 1697–1709. 39. Kistler WM, De Zeeuw CI (2002) Dynamical working memory and timed responses: the role of reverberating loops in the olivo-cerebellar system. Neural Comput 14: 2597–2626. 40. LeDoux MS, Hurst DC, Lorden JF (1998) Single-unit activity of cerebellar nuclear cells in the awake genetically dystonic rat. Neuroscience 86: 533–545. 41. Gauck V, Thomann M, Jaeger D, Borst A (2001) Spatial distribution of low- and high-voltage-activated calcium currents in neurons of the deep cerebellar nuclei. J Neurosci 21: RC158. 42. Koekkoek SKE, Yamaguchi K, Milojkovic BA, Dortland BR, Ruigrok TJH, et al. (2005) Deletion of FMR1 in Purkinje cells enhances parallel fiber LTD, enlarges spines, and attenuates eyelid conditioning in a manner which phenocopies human Fragile X syndrome. Neuron 47: 339–352. 43. Koch C (1999) Biophysics of computation: Information processing in single neurons. New York: Oxford University Press. 44. Steuber V, Mittmann W, Hoebeek FE, Silver RA, De Zeeuw CI, et al. (2007) Cerebellar LTD and pattern recognition by Purkinje cells. Neuron 54: 121–136. 17. Hausser M, Clark BA (1997) Tonic synaptic inhibition modulates neuronal output pattern and spatiotemporal synaptic integration. Neuron 19: 665–678. 18. Raman IM, Bean BP (1999) Ionic currents underlying spontaneous action potentials in isolated cerebellar Purkinje neurons. J Neurosci 19: 1663–1674. 19. Shin SL, De Schutter E (2006) Dynamic synchronization of Purkinje cell simple spikes. J Neurophysiol 96: 3485–3491. 20. Holt GR, Softky WR, Koch C, Douglas RJ (1996) Comparison of discharge variability in vitro and in vivo in cat visual cortex neurons. J Neurophysiol 75: 1806–1814. 21. Vos BP, Maex R, Volny-Luraghi A, De Schutter E (1999) Parallel fibers synchronize spontaneous activity in cerebellar Golgi cells. J NeuroSci 19: RC6. 22. Eggermont JJ, Smith GM (1995) Rate covariance dominates spontaneous cortical unit-pair correlograms. Neuroreport 6: 2125–2128. 23. Maex R, Vos BP, De Schutter E (2000) Weak common parallel fibre synapses explain the loose synchrony observed between rat cerebellar golgi cells. J Physiol 523Pt 1, 175–192. 24. Loewenstein Y, Mahon S, Chadderton P, Kitamura K, Sompolinsky H, et al. (2005) Bistability of cerebellar Purkinje cells modulated by sensory stimulation. Nat Neurosci 8: 202–211. 25. Shin SL, Rotter S, Aertsen A, De Schutter E (2007) Stochastic description of complex and simple spike firing in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Eur J Neurosci 25: 785–794. 26. Pedroarena CM, Schwarz C (2003) Efficacy and short-term plasticity at GABAergic synapses between Purkinje and cerebellar nuclei neurons. J Neurophysiol 89: 704–715. 27. Telgkamp P, Raman IM (2002) Depression of inhibitory synaptic transmission between Purkinje cells and neurons of the cerebellar nuclei. J Neurosci 22: 8447–8457. 28. Telgkamp P, Padgett DE, Ledoux VA, Woolley CS, Raman IM (2004) Maintenance of high-frequency transmission at purkinje to cerebellar nuclear synapses by spillover from boutons with multiple release sites. Neuron 41: 113–126. 29. Abbott LF, Regehr WG (2004) Synaptic computation. Nature 431: 796–803. 30. De Zeeuw CI, Wylie DR, DiGiorgi PL, Simpson JI (1994) Projections of individual Purkinje cells of identified zones in the flocculus to the vestibular and cerebellar nuclei in the rabbit. J Comp Neurol 349: 428–447. PLoS ONE | www.plosone.org 9 May 2007 | Issue 5 | e485