Self and Identity, 4: 59–68, 2005
Copyright # 2005 Psychology Press
ISSN: 1529-8868 print/1529-8876 online
DOI: 10.1080/13576500444000218
Narcissism and Adult Attachment:
A Multivariate Approach
KATHY SMOLEWSKA
KENNETH L. DION
University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
The primary purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between
maladaptive narcissism, attachment anxiety, and attachment avoidance. The
secondary objective of the present research was to explore further the differences
between overt and covert forms of narcissistic vulnerability. Female participants
(N = 171) completed measures of adult attachment, overt narcissism, and covert
narcissism. Canonical correlation analysis (CCA) explored the multivariate
relationship between overt and covert narcissism, on one hand, and adult attachment
dimensions of anxiety and avoidance, on the other hand. CCA indicated the two
linear combinations of variables overlapped significantly and shared about a quarter
of their variance in common. The most important variable within the narcissism set
was covert narcissism; and within the adult attachment set, both anxiety and
avoidance were important, but the former more than the latter. The implications of
the present findings are discussed in the context of past and future research on
personality, attachment, and the experience of emotions.
According to theory and research in developmental and social psychology,
attachment refers to characteristic patterns of experiencing relationships that stem
from beliefs about the self and others originating in early childhood. Attachment
theorists believe that the quality of the initial bond between caregiver and child plays
a powerful role in determining the quality of that child’s future intimate relationships
as a young adult and beyond. Within the personality and self-concept literature,
early childhood experiences have also been implicated in the development of
narcissism. The narcissism construct stems from psychodynamic theory and is most
frequently described as a personality trait defined by a combination of grandiosity
and vulnerability. Although narcissism and attachment have traditionally been
examined from different perspectives, recent research suggests the two constructs
may share a similar underlying structure (Brennan & Shaver, 1998; Feintuch, 1998).
Pistole (1995) proposed that the different forms of insecure attachment, characterized by varying degrees of avoidance and/or anxiety, may actually have the same
purpose in that they are manifestations of defense mechanisms employed by
individuals high in narcissistic vulnerability. While a few studies have looked at the
Received 16 July 2003; accepted 13 July 2004
Preparation of this paper was facilitated by a research grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities
Research Council of Canada to the second author.
Kathy Smolewska is now at the Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Canada.
Address correspondence to Kathy Smolewska, Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo,
Waterloo, Ontario, N2L 3G1, Canada. E-mail:
[email protected]
59
60
K. Smolewska & K. L. Dion
relationship between attachment and narcissism, results so far have been somewhat
inconclusive.
One reason for the lack of clear findings may be the failure to differentiate between
two forms of maladaptive narcissism. According to Wink (1991), one form
encompasses individuals who are overtly self-absorbed (i.e., overt narcissism), while
the other characterizes individuals who are covertly self-absorbed (i.e., covert
narcissism). While both forms have an underlying sense of entitlement and a tendency
towards exploitativeness, overt narcissism is characterized by a need for dominance, a
lack of affiliative concerns, self-reliance, and suspicions of other people, while covert
narcissism is typified more by feelings of unworthiness, hypersensitivity, negative
emotionality, and self-focused attention (Hendin & Cheek, 1997).
Maladaptive Narcissism and Insecure Attachment
Self theorists believe parental feedback lacking in empathy and attention may
eventually undermine inherently healthy narcissistic potential and lead a child to
remain fixated at an infantile, self-centered, ‘‘grandiose’’ stage of development,
constantly craving attention (Watson, Hickman, Morris, Milliron, & Whiting, 1993).
According to Kohut (1971), overt narcissism results when the rudimentary self
formed in childhood fails to be integrated properly with the rest of the personality,
owing to inadequate mirroring (i.e., encouraging praise and expressions of acceptance
that inform the child it is ‘‘good’’) by the parent. In an attempt to resume the
psychological growth process and fulfill unmet infantile needs, the narcissistic
individual continues to express grandiosity and exhibitionism into adulthood.
Although the covert narcissist has the same exhibitionistic need for attention,
grandiose fantasy is less conscious and only apparent with close contact (Kernberg,
1975). The covert narcissist has a greater sense of vulnerability, characterized by
extreme sensitivity, ineffective emotional regulation, and feelings of inferiority
(Hendin & Cheek, 1997). According to Kohut (1971), the covert narcissist’s inability
to regulate his/her emotions stems from parental failures in responding to troubled
emotions expressed by the child. This deficit in supportive parenting may result in the
lack of appropriate strategies for dealing with intense emotions and excessive reliance
on defense mechanisms to regulate one’s self-concept and emotions (Pistole, 1995).
These defense mechanisms are illustrated in Ainsworth’s (1970, 1989) study of
infant and childhood attachment styles in an anxiety-provoking situation. By
temporarily separating young children from their mother, Ainsworth was able to
observe children’s reactions in response to the mother’s return and found that
children could be classified into categories of secure, avoidant, and anxious/
ambivalent attachment styles. Defense mechanisms were evident in the two latter
styles: Avoidant subjects ignored the mother upon return and failed to seek her
comfort when distressed; while anxious/ambivalent children showed both clinging
and distancing behaviors.
Attachment and Narcissism in Romantic Relationships
Following Ainsworth’s early work (e.g., 1970), Hazan and Shaver (1987) identified
three adult romantic attachment styles (secure, anxious – ambivalent, and avoidant).
These have since been reconceptualized into a four-category model (secure,
preoccupied, fearful, and dismissing) based on a two dimensional space defined by
anxiety and avoidance (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998). The dimensions of
Narcissism and Adult Attachment
61
attachment anxiety (hereafter AAnx) and attachment avoidance (hereafter AAvoid)
help to illustrate how the two forms of maladaptive narcissism and adult romantic
attachment may intersect within the attachment theory construct of internal working
models. The AAnx dimension is associated with preoccupation, jealousy/fear of
abandonment, and fear of rejection. Thus, AAnx is characterized by a negative
affective experience associated with perceptions of threat, which is believed to stem
from the negative model of self that arises from inaccessible and/or unresponsive
parenting (Bowlby, 1973). With respect to narcissistic vulnerability and romantic
attachment, AAnx may relate to higher levels of covert narcissism because covertly
narcissistic individuals are reportedly less tolerant of stressful events and more prone
to being overwhelmed by distress and experiencing unpleasant emotions (Mikulincer
& Florian, 1998). AAnx individuals may feel inferior as romantic partners and
unworthy of the affections of others, which creates a sense of anxiety; or they may
feel anxious because they have had experiences with undependable or unpredictable
attachment figures in the past.
AAvoid is associated with early childhood experiences with rejecting and/or
inexpressive caregivers and encompasses avoidance of intimacy, discomfort with
closeness, and excessive self-reliance (Brennan et al., 1998). The covert narcissist’s
low threshold for threat (i.e., anxiety-provoking situations), combined with mixed
perceptions of support, would contribute to frequent activation of the attachment
system, leading to distress. Thus, avoidance may be a coping strategy for the
hypersensitivity of covert narcissists.
In the context of social interaction and relationships, overt narcissism is
characterized by a need for dominance, a lack of affiliative concerns, self-reliance,
and suspiciousness toward others (Wink, 1991). When someone chronically lacks
support from an attachment figure, he/she may develop an increased threshold for
experiencing negative emotions or perceiving attachment needs and thus exhibit
what Bowlby (1973) called ‘‘compulsive self-reliance.’’ In the case of the overt
narcissist, the importance of social ties is dismissed, and interpersonal exploitation
and exhibitionism are employed to create an illusion of self-importance. Thus, the
prospect of romantic relationships may not be associated with high levels of AAnx
for the overt narcissist. However, although they may not report feeling anxious,
Robbins and Dupont (1992) suggested that a primary reason for their dismissal of
close relationships may be the threat relationships pose to their self-concept, thus
leading to high levels of AAvoid.
In sum, the following predictions were made. It was hypothesized that AAnx
would be related solely to covert narcissism. Since overt narcissism is characterized
by dismissal of the importance of attachment, levels of overt narcissism were not
hypothesized to relate to AAnx. In contrast, the level of AAvoid was expected to be
augmented for higher scorers on both covert and overt narcissism, since for both it
can be viewed as a mechanism of psychological defense.
The present study sought to examine how the combination of covert and overt
narcissism was multivariately related to adult attachment dimensions for a sample
of Canadian university women. Toward this end, we employed canonical
correlation analysis (hereafter CCA), a technique that assesses the relationship
between two linear combinations of variables. CCA is ‘‘one of the most general
of the multivariate techniques,’’ of which multiple regression, discriminant
function analysis, and MANOVA are special cases (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996,
p. 196), as is correspondence analysis in the case of only categorical variables
(Clausen, 1998).
62
K. Smolewska & K. L. Dion
Psychologists increasingly employ CCA when both independent and dependent
variables are multivariate; and the interest is in how the two sets of variables as a
whole, rather than singly on one or both sides, relate to one another (Shell &
Husman, 2001). Relationships between variables may differ when examined as part
of a multivariate variable set compared to when they are examined separately or in
isolation from other related but nonredundant variables. CCA ‘‘summarizes the
between domains covariance in as few statistically reliable dimensions as possible,’’
thus providing an elegant and powerful solution to the question of the multivariate
relationship between two sets of variables (Wingard, Huba, & Bentler, 1979, p. 135).
Method
Participants
Participants were 171 female undergraduates enrolled in introductory psychology at
the University of Toronto. Respondents were between 17 and 39 years of age, with a
median of 19 years. Most participants (93.5%) classified themselves as single or
dating someone regularly.
Procedure
The present study was advertised on the psychology course web page as part of a
larger study concerning perceptions of one’s familial environment, childhood
experiences, and attitudes towards self and others. Participants were tested in group
sessions of up to six people, with people seated at separate desks. To maintain
respondents’ anonymity, consent forms were signed and collected prior to
administering the questionnaire, which the participants had 50 minutes to complete.
Upon its completion, they were individually debriefed and any questions and
concerns were addressed. For taking part, students received a point toward the final
mark.
Measures
The following three scales were used to assess the constructs of interest: (1) the
Hypersensitive Narcissism Scale, which measures levels of hypersensitive or covert
narcissism; (2) the Narcissistic Personality Inventory, which measures levels of overt
narcissism; and (3) the Experience of Close Relationships Scale, which measures
anxiety and avoidance dimensions of attachment. These measures were assessed for
reliability by computing coefficient alphas (hereafter a) and average inter-item
correlations (hereafter AIC). A scale is generally considered to be internally
consistent when its alpha is .70 or higher and the average inter-item correlation is .20
or better (Briggs & Cheek, 1986).
Hypersensitive Narcissism Scale. The ten-item Hypersensitive Narcissism Scale
(HSNS; Hendin & Cheek, 1997) was derived from Murray’s (1938, as cited in
Hendin & Cheek, 1997) Narcism Scale and has been found to possess appropriate
psychometric qualities to be useful as a measure of covert narcissism. Using three
independent samples of college students, Hendin and Cheek (1997) found that the
HSNS and an MMPI-based composite measure of covert narcissism showed similar
Narcissism and Adult Attachment
63
patterns of correlation with the Big Five Inventory, and both showed near zero
correlations with the Narcissistic Personality Inventory, a measure of overt
narcissism.
Items on this measure had a Likert scale format scored from 1 (disagree strongly)
to 5 (agree strongly). Our reliability analysis of the HSNS showed that deleting two
items improved reliability. Thus, an 8-item index of covert narcissism was created,
with a = .70 and AIC = .23.
Narcissistic Personality Inventory. Overt narcissism was assessed with an abridged
version of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI, Raskin & Hall, 1979),
comprised of 40 forced-choice items. The NPI is the pre-eminent measure of
narcissism in research on the normal range of individual differences, and its validity
has been examined in numerous studies. These studies have shown that the NPI does
assess some important aspects of the multidimensional construct of narcissism,
particularly the overt form (Hendin & Cheek, 1997).
Since overt narcissism is known to consist of several dimensions, principal axis
factoring was employed to identify sets of intercorrelated items. The results of
parallel analysis were used to determine the number of factors to retain. Parallel
analysis is a statistical procedure in which eigenvalues derived from random data
based on the number of items in the measure and the number of respondents are
compared to eigenvalues produced by factor analysis of actual data (Longman,
Cota, Holden, & Fekken, 1989). Based on parallel analysis, the principal axis
solution was varimax-rotated to four factors. However, these four factors accounted
for only 24% of the variance, with substantial overlap in items between the factors
(i.e., there were several doublets). As a result, we decided that a unidimensional index
would better represent the construct. Thus, overt narcissism was assessed by a 40item NPI index with an a = 0.80 and AIC = .21.
Experience of Close Relationships Scale. Adult attachment was assessed using the
36-item Experience of Close Relationships Scale (Brennan et al., 1998). Participants
answered items only if they were presently involved in a romantic relationship or had
been in the past. Respondents referring to a past relationship were instructed to
answer the questions for the most recent one. Participants indicated how true each
statement was of their relationship, by selecting a number from 1 (strongly disagree)
to 7 (strongly agree). The measure was scored in accordance with Brennan et al.’s
(1998) instructions for the two dimensions of AAvoid and AAnx. The AAnx index
was composed of 18 items with a = .90 and AIC = .33. The AAvoid index consisted
of 16 items, with a = .93 and AIC = .33.
Results
Bivariate Correlation Analyses
Bivariate correlations were computed for the four variables of interest (i.e., overt
narcissism, covert narcissism, AAvoid, AAnx). The bivariate correlation coefficient
between overt and covert narcissism was insignificant (r = .075, df = 166), as were
the bivariate correlation coefficients between overt narcissism and AAvoid and
AAnx (r = .124, df = 148 and r = – .037, df = 148 respectively). The bivariate
correlation coefficient between AAvoid and AAnx was significant (r = .172,
64
K. Smolewska & K. L. Dion
df = 150, p = .03). The largest bivariate correlation coefficients were between covert
narcissism and AAvoid and AAnx. Specifically, covert narcissism was positively
correlated with both AAvoid and AAnx (r = .238, df = 150, p 5 .01 and r = .447,
df = 150, p 5 .01, respectively).
Canonical Correlation Analyses
Canonical correlation analysis (CCA) was performed between the set of narcissism
variables (i.e., overt narcissism and covert narcissism), on one hand, and adult
attachment variables (i.e., avoidance and anxiety), on the other hand. We used the
canonical correlation syntax routine in SPSS Windows to perform the CCA. The
first canonical correlation, rC1, was .485, indicating a 23% overlap or ‘‘common
variance’’ between the two linear combinations of variables. The first canonical
variate was significant, w2 (4) = 42.08, p 5 .001, indicating a significant degree of
overlap or common variance between the two domains or linear combinations of
variable sets. Once the first canonical correlation was removed, however, no further
reliable relationship between the two variable sets was evident, as indicated by a
nonsignificant w2 (1) = 2.85, p = .09, for the second canonical correlation, rC2 = .14.
Although the latter canonical correlation is marginally significant, we did not
interpret it. According to Tabachnick and Fidell (1996, p. 221), ‘‘most researchers do
not interpret . . . a canonical correlation lower than .30, because [it represents] less
than 10% overlap in variance.’’
Accordingly, only the first canonical variate and its associated statistical output
are presented in Table 1. Table 1 presents the standardized canonical coefficients, the
canonical loadings, the percent of variance of the variable set accounted for by its
own canonical variate, and the redundancy (i.e., the percent of variance in one
variable set accounted for by the ‘‘opposite canonical variate’’ for the other variable
set). As shown in Table 1, the first canonical variate accounted for nearly 50% of the
variance in the set of narcissism variables and slightly more than 55% of the variance
in the adult attachment variable set. As well, the redundancy values indicate the
TABLE 1
Canonical Correlation Analysis of Narcissism and Adult Attachment
First Canonical Variate
Narcissism Set
Overt narcissism
Covert narcissism
Percent variance
Redundancy
Adult Attachment Set
Avoidance
Anxiety
Percent variance
Redundancy
Canonical correlation
a
Coefficienta
Loadingb
– .128
1.00
.492
.116
.004
.992
.287
.904
.567
.133
.485
.461
.959
Standardized Canonical Coefficient.
Canonical Loading (aka Structure Coefficients).
b
Narcissism and Adult Attachment
65
proportion of variance in one variable set accounted for by the other variable set. In
both cases, redundancies were between 10 – 15%.
Canonical variates are defined by both the canonical coefficients and the
canonical loadings (also known as structure coefficients). Canonical coefficients and
loadings potentially provide independent but complementary information about the
multivariate relationship between two sets of variables, especially when variables in a
set are not highly correlated (see Shell & Husman, 2001). Canonical coefficients
indicate the weighting of each variable in the discriminant function defining the
canonical variate. Like a regression weight, canonical coefficients indicate the
independent contribution of each variable to the canonical variate with the other
variables in the set partialled out. The pattern of weightings, however, can be
arbitrary when variables in the set are highly correlated. In such instances, canonical
loadings are preferred for interpretation. When the variables in a set are not highly
correlated, however, the original variables have more of a chance to contribute
independently to defining the canonical variate. In the present study, the bivariate
correlation was .08 for the two narcissism variables and only .17 for anxiety and
avoidance in the attachment set. Since these intercorrelations are weak, the canonical
coefficients are used to interpret the canonical variate. Finally, since standardized
canonical coefficients are not scaled within a bounded range, no particular ‘‘absolute
value’’ represents a meaningful contribution. Accordingly, a value of .20 was taken
as a cutoff for interpretive purposes, following Shell and Husman (2001).
Canonical coefficients. As shown in Table 1, three of the four variables in the two
variable sets contributed meaningfully to the canonical variate. Covert narcissism
and anxiety attachment contributed most strongly. Avoidance attachment had a
smaller but still notable contribution to the canonical variate.
Canonical loadings. The loadings yielded a generally similar pattern to that of the
canonical coefficients. Canonical loadings are correlations between participants’
scores on the original variables and the canonical variate. Loadings indicate which
variables are associated with the canonical variate and are analogous to factor
loadings in factor analysis. Using a cutoff value of .30, three of the four variables
loaded on the canonical variate, with covert narcissism and anxiety attachment
having by far the highest loadings, and avoidance attachment having a smaller
though still quite substantial loading. Taking the sign of the loadings into account
provides further information. High scores on covert narcissism were associated with
high scores on both anxiety and avoidance attachment.
Discussion
Nearly a quarter of the variance was shared between the covert narcissism and
attachment domains. Knowing a person’s score for covert narcissism has predictive
value for estimating individual differences in adult attachment, especially anxiety
attachment, and vice versa.
This multivariate relationship supports conceptualizations from research and
theorizing on narcissism as well as attachment. It is mainly the covert aspect of
narcissism, reflecting vulnerability and hypersensitivity to rejection by others, that
relates to adult attachment, rather than overt narcissism, which reflects selfaggrandizement and grandiosity. Interpersonally, overt narcissists are defensive,
cold, and emotionally detached. Thus, although they may not have an underlying
66
K. Smolewska & K. L. Dion
positive self-concept, they manage to cope by dismissing interpersonal goals and
focusing on self-centered goals relating to power and entitlement. Covert narcissists,
on the other hand, may experience high levels of anxiety in romantic relationships
because they have not learned to cope with their negative self-concept and fear
others will see them as unworthy of their affections. Hendin and Cheek (1997)
concluded that along with individual differences in sensitivity to rejection (Downey
& Feldman, 1996) to which it is likely related, covert narcissism was part of a more
general personality construct of psychological insecurity. Downey, Freitas,
Michaelis, and Khouri (1998) showed that women high in rejection sensitivity
behaved negatively toward a male romantic partner during interaction so as to elicit
rejection and fulfill their prophecy of expected rejection. The covert narcissist might
likewise be prone to such self-fulfilling prophecies in close or intimate relationships.
Levy, Ayduk, and Downey (2001) speculated about ways to break the rejection
sensitivity cycle, through providing supportive social relationships and other
strategies, which may also benefit covert narcissists.
The stronger association of covert narcissism to anxiety attachment than
avoidance attachment is also in keeping with contemporary attachment theory
(Fraley & Shaver, 2000). Anxiety attachment reflects a proneness to anxiety and
vigilance concerning rejection and abandonment by others. By contrast, avoidance
attachment, which primarily reflects discomfort with closeness to others, should
relate less strongly to the dynamics of covert narcissism. However, the small
association found between covert narcissism and avoidance attachment suggests that
avoidance may be used as a defense strategy in order to protect the self from anxiety
stemming from fear of rejection (Pistole, 1995). Thus, by avoiding relationships, the
covert narcissist is able to conceal attachment-related distress and protect a fragile
self-concept. Fraley and Shaver’s (2000) interpretation of attachment dimensions in
terms of emotional and behavioral regulation suggests that covert narcissists might
have particular difficulty regulating their emotions and behavior in the context of
attachment relationships and perhaps interpersonal relationships generally—an idea
perhaps worth exploring in future research.
Our results lend further credence to previous findings of two distinct forms of
narcissistic vulnerability (Feintuch, 1998; Wink, 1991) that may be distinguished in
terms of their underlying cognitive-affective structure and differential associations
with working models of attachment. This conclusion is supported by a recent study
examining the two subtypes of pathological narcissism in relation to attachment
orientations and interpersonal functioning. Consistent with our findings, Dickinson
and Pincus (2003) found that vulnerable narcissism was associated with high ratings
on avoidant personality disorder and fearful attachment, which is characterized by
anxiety and avoidance in relationships. They also found that overt narcissism was
associated with secure or dismissive attachment, both reflecting low anxiety in
relationships.
Limitations of our research include its correlational design (which precludes
identifying causes), exclusive use of self-report measures, and having a homogeneous
college student sample of women as participants. Future research should examine
the relationship between narcissism and attachment in more diverse samples with
broader variation of age, gender, and cultural background.
Future research should also consider the role of attachment and narcissism in the
development of interpersonal problems and psychopathology. Highly sensitive
individuals avoid novel, unpredictable situations because they do not deal well with
the distress from uncertainty (Aron & Aron, 1997). Since early phases of close
Narcissism and Adult Attachment
67
relationships are often unpredictable and uncertain, hypersensitive people may
experience emotional conflict as extremely distressing, evoking both unpleasant
emotions and anxiety (Pistole, 1995). As a result, not only may high levels of covert
narcissism interfere with romantic relationships but their inability to use social
resources may put them at a greater risk for psychological distress (Emmons &
Colby, 1995).
By examining how covert narcissism and adult attachment are linked to clinical
issues pertaining to self-worth and relationships, strategies can be developed to
better address problems of people with dysfunctional narcissism and attachment in a
therapeutic setting.
References
Ainsworth, M. S., & Bell, S. M. (1970). Attachment, exploration, and separation: Illustrated
by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. Child Development, 41, 49 – 67.
Ainsworth, M. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44, 709 – 716.
Aron, E. N., & Aron, A. (1997). Sensory-processing sensitivity and its relation to introversion
and emotionality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 345 – 368.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Separation: Anxiety and anger (Vol. 2). New York: Basic Books.
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult
romantic attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.),
Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46 – 76). New York: Guilford.
Brennan, K. A., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Attachment styles and personality disorders: Their
connections to each other and to parental divorce, parental death, and perceptions of
parental caregiving. Journal of Personality, 66, 835 – 878.
Briggs, S. R., & Cheek, J. M. (1986). The role of factor analysis in the development and
evaluation of personality scales. Journal of Personality, 54, 106 – 148.
Clausen, S. E. (1998). Applied correspondence analysis: An introduction. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Dickinson, K. A., & Pincus, A. L. (2003). Interpersonal analysis of grandiose and vulnerable
narcissism. Journal of Personality Disorders, 17, 188 – 207.
Downey, G., & Feldman, S. I. (1996). Implications of rejection sensitivity for intimate
relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1327 – 1343.
Downey, G., Freitas, A. L., Michaelis, B., & Khouri, H. (1998). The self-fulfilling prophecy in
close relationships: Rejection sensitivity and rejection by romantic partners. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 545 – 560.
Emmons, R. A., & Colby, P. M. (1995). Emotional conflict and well-being: Relation to
perceived availability, daily utilization, and observer reports of social support. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 947 – 959.
Feintuch, B. (1998). Adult attachment, narcissism, shame, and defensiveness. Unpublished PhD,
Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI.
Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult romantic attachment: Theoretical developments,
emerging controversies, and unanswered questions. Review of General Psychology, 4,
132 – 154.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511 – 524.
Hendin, H. M., & Cheek, J. M. (1997). Assessing hypersensitive narcissism: A reexamination
of Murray’s Narcism Scale. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 588 – 599.
Kernberg, O. (1975). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. New York: Jason
Aronson.
Kohut, H. (1971). The analysis of the self. New York: International Universities Press.
68
K. Smolewska & K. L. Dion
Levy, S. R., Ayduk, O., & Downey, G. (2001). The role of rejection sensitivity in people’s
relationships with significant others and valued social groups. In M. Leary (Ed.),
Interpersonal rejection (Ch. 10, pp. 251 – 289). New York: Oxford University Press.
Longman, S. R., Cota, A. A., Holden, R. R., & Fekken, G. C. (1989). A regression equation
for the parallel analysis criterion in principal components analysis: Mean and 95th
percentile eigenvalues. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 24, 59 – 69.
Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (1998). The relationship between adult attachment styles and
emotional and cognitive reactions to stressful events. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes
(Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 143 – 165). New York: Guilford.
Pistole, M. C. (1995). Adult attachment style and narcissistic vulnerability. Psychoanalytic
Psychology, 12, 115 – 126.
Raskin, R., & Hall, C. S. (1979). A narcissistic personality inventory. Psychological Reports,
45, 590.
Robbins, S. B., & Dupont, P. (1992). Narcissistic needs of the self and perceptions of
interpersonal behavior. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 39, 462 – 467.
Shell, D. F., & Husman, J. (2001). The multivariate dimensionality of personal control and
future time perspective beliefs in achievement and self-regulation. Contemporary
Educational Psychology, 26, 481 – 506.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (3rd ed.). New York:
HarperCollins.
Watson, P. J., Hickman, S. E., Morris, R. J., Milliron, J. T., & Whiting, L. (1993). Narcissism,
self-esteem, and parental nurturance. The Journal of Psychology, 129, 61 – 73.
Wingard, J. A., Huba, G. J., & Bentler, P. M. (1979). The relationship of personality structure
to patterns of adolescent substance use. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 14, 131 – 143.
Wink, P. (1991). Two faces of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61,
590 – 597.