Journal of Health Economics 72 (2020) 102317
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Journal of Health Economics
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/econbase
Social capital and health: a meta-analysis
Xindong Xue a,b , W. Robert Reed c,∗ , Andrea Menclova c
a
b
c
School of Public Administration, Zhongnan University of Economics and Law, China
Collaborative Innovation Center for Social Governance of Hubei Province, China
College of Business and Law, University of Canterbury, New Zealand
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Received 4 January 2019
Received in revised form 2 December 2019
Accepted 12 March 2020
JEL classification:
B49
C49
I10
I31
Keywords:
Social capital
Health
Meta-analysis
Mental health
Physical health
Self-reported health
a b s t r a c t
The relationship between social capital and health has received extensive attention in fields
such as public health, medicine, epidemiology, gerontology and other health-related disciplines. In contrast, the economics literature on this subject is relatively small. To address this
research gap, we investigate the cross-disciplinary empirical literature using meta-analysis.
We analyze 12,778 estimates from 470 studies. Our analysis finds that social capital is significantly related to a variety of positive health outcomes. However, the effect sizes are
consistently very small. This finding is robust across different types of social capital (e.g.,
cognitive, structural, bonding, bridging, linking), and for many different measures of health
outcomes (e.g., mortality, disease/illnesses, depression). The small effects that we estimate
cast doubt on recent initiatives to promote health through social capital such as those by
the WHO, the OECD, and US Healthy People 2020.
© 2020 Published by Elsevier B.V.
1. Introduction
This study examines the literature on social capital and
health. It has long been recognized that social capital plays
an important role in economic affairs. A recent search
on Web of Science identified over 1000 studies in economics journals in which social capital appears in the title.
Research on social capital has been published in the American Economic Review (Guiso et al., 2004; Karlan, 2005), the
Quarterly Journal of Economics (Knack and Keefer, 1997;
Glaeser et al., 2000), the Economic Journal (Bowles and
Gintis, 2002; Glaeser et al., 2002; Durlauf, 2002; Helliwell,
2006), and other top economics journals. The topic contin-
∗ Corresponding author.
E-mail address:
[email protected] (W.R. Reed).
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhealeco.2020.102317
0167-6296/© 2020 Published by Elsevier B.V.
ues to be of interest to economists (McCoy et al., 2019; Hoi
et al., 2019; de Vaan et al., 2019d; Andini and Andini, 2018;
and Wang, 2019).
However, most of the economic research on social capital has focused on areas such as economic growth (Knack
and Keefer, 1997; Algan and Cahuc, 2010), financial development (Guiso et al., 2004), political governance (Putnam,
1993; Nannicini et al., 2013) and formation of large firms
and organizations (La Porta et al., 1997). While some economic research has focused on health, the literature is
relatively small. This is surprising, not only because social
capital has been found to be an important determinant of
economic outcomes in other areas, but also because there
is a voluminous literature on social capital and health outside of economics, particularly in public health, medicine,
epidemiology, gerontology and other health-related disciplines.
2
X. Xue, W.R. Reed and A. Menclova / Journal of Health Economics 72 (2020) 102317
This study uses meta-analysis to aggregate empirical
findings in the literature on social capital and health.1 Our
goal is to assess the overall economic impact of social capital on health and determine whether it is “large” or “small”.
Given the broad scope of both “social capital” and “health”,
we also want to dig deeper into the overall relationship to
explore whether certain types of social capital are particularly impactful for certain types of health outcomes.
Our primary motivation is to determine whether this
is a subject that should attract more attention from
economists. If studies from other disciplines, and the extant
economics literature on this topic, have found that social
capital is an important determinant of health, then the
dearth of economic research in this area represents a gap
that should be addressed. On the other hand, if the conclusion from existing studies is that social capital is not
an important determinant of health, then this justifies the
relative lack of attention this subject has received in the
economics discipline. Further, it would call into question
recent initiatives to promote health through social capital by the WHO, the OECD, and US Healthy People 2020
(Rocco and Suhrcke, 2012; Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention, 2010; Keeley, 2007).
This study proceeds as follows. Section 2 reviews the
economics literature on social capital. Section 3 explains
our use of meta-analysis and our strategy for combining
diverse estimates from different studies. Section 4 presents
and discusses the data that we use in our analysis. Section
5 presents initial estimates of the “overall” effect of social
capital on health, along with the results of a commonly used
test for publication bias. Section 6 uses meta-regression
to identify the factors that affect the size of the estimated
effects of social capital on health. It also takes a closer look
at finer categories of social capital and health to see if there
are some kinds of social capital that may affect some kinds
of health outcomes, even if the overall effect is small. Section 7 concludes by summarizing our results and drawing
out implications for public policy and further study.
monitoring costs. Further, social capital in the form of
trust can reduce transaction costs, facilitating economic
exchange and contributing to economic growth.
The last mechanism has received the most empirical
attention. LaPorta et al. (1997) show that trust improves the
performance of large organizations. Zak and Knack (2001)
present empirical evidence that trust is significantly related
to economic growth. Their findings are corroborated by
Beugelsdijk et al. (2004). Algan and Cahuc (2010) further
substantiate the relationship between trust and economic
growth by using “inherited trust” of descendants of US
immigrants to establish a causal link between trust and
growth.
Another area that has received much attention is the
relationship between trust and economic institutions.
Berggren and Jordahl (2006) find that legal structure and
security of property rights increase trust. Alesina, and
Giuliano (2015) provide a recent summary of how culture, trust, and institutions interact to affect economic
outcomes. Relatedly, Guiso, Sapienza, & Zingales (2004)
and Tabellini (2010) find a strong link between culture, of
which trust is a major component, and trade and economic
development, respectively. Overall, there is broad consensus in the literature that the economic benefits of social
capital in general – and trust in particular – are large.
In contrast, the role of social capital in health is more
opaque. Folland (2008) identifies a number of avenues by
which social capital can affect health. Social capital may
reduce stress through supportive relationships, trust, and
the benefits of socializing. Stress reduction, in turn, positively affects health. Social capital in the form of socializing
may make it easier to obtain (health-related) information.
Further, having strong social ties may encourage individuals to make more “responsible” choices as their health
and wellbeing are of importance to others whom they care
about.
In Folland’s (2008) formal model, social capital affects
an individual’s utility U both directly, and indirectly via its
effects on health:
2. Literature on the economic effects of Social
Capital
U = U(S, H, X)
As noted above, there is a large literature that investigates the relationship between social capital and economic
activity. A number of channels have been proposed for
this relationship (Paldam and Svendsen, 2000). Social capital may enter the production function directly, alongside
physical capital and human capital; e.g., in the form of
consumer networks, reputation, and goodwill. Social capital may reduce free-riding and thus lessen the need for
regulation in settings that require cooperation, reducing
H = H(S, C(S))
1
Previous meta-analyses of social capital and health have been published outside of economics (De Silva et al., 2005; Holt-Lunstad et al.,
2010; Gilbert et al., 2013; Nyqvist et al., 2014). A working paper version of
this manuscript describes the value-added of our analysis. Among other
things, our study far exceeds these previous efforts in scope and detail.
Further, previous studies have directly combined disparate estimates. In
contrast, we translate estimates into partial correlation coefficients, which
is a more appropriate method for comparing and combining diverse estimates.
(1.a)
where
(1.b)
and S is social capital, H is health, X are other consumption
goods, and C are all other health inputs.
Social capital in the form of socializing may improve
health directly. For example, as noted above, supportive
relationships can improve health by reducing stress, so
that ∂H > 0. Social capital can also affect health indirectly.
∂S
For example, strong family relationships may encourage
an individual to obtain regular medical checkups, C, so that
∂H ∂C > 0. Alternatively, a desire to belong to a peer group
∂C ∂S
which shares cigarettes may encourage an individual to
smoke, C, ∂H ∂C < 0. As a result, the overall effect of social
∂C ∂S
capital on health is theoretically ambiguous.
Other formal models of the relationship between
health and social capital are presented by Costa-Font
and Mladovsky (2008) and Laporte (2014). Costa-Font
and Mladovsky (2008) model the effects of social capital on health via the effects of peers on health-related
X. Xue, W.R. Reed and A. Menclova / Journal of Health Economics 72 (2020) 102317
preferences. This corresponds to the effect of S on C,
and subsequently H, in Folland’s model above. However, Costa-Font and Mladovsky (2008) extend this logic
by modelling the propagation of preferences within a
group through a social multiplier effect. Their approach
allows for the dissemination of both health-improving
(e.g., physical activity, medical checkups, vaccinations) and
health-retarding (e.g., social smoking, drinking, and the
obesity epidemic) behaviors within a social group.
Laporte (2014) extends the classic Grossman (1972)
model of investment in health to include social capital.
She allows for two, complementary types of social capital,
private social capital, in which the individual invests over
time, and public social capital, which is exogenously determined. One consequence of the model is that increased
geographical mobility on the part of health consumers can
induce them to invest less in private social capital. Communities may respond by investing more in public social
capital. This highlights the role of public policy in social
capital. Folland at al. (2014) also explore the role of public
policy in the provision of social capital.
The above provides ample theoretical reasons for
believing that social capital can be an important input to
health. It also points out that social capital’s influence need
not always be positive. Either way, the size of the economic
impact of social capital on health is an empirical question.
The next section explains the methodology we will use to
answer this question.
3. Methodology
We use meta-analysis to aggregate empirical findings
in the cross-disciplinary literature on social capital and
health. To be included in our meta-analysis, a study must
estimate the “effect” that social capital has on health. As
will be discussed below, there are a variety of estimation
procedures and model specifications that studies have used
to do this. Conceptually, we can represent these efforts with
a linear model that regresses a measure of health (H) on a
measure of social capital (S), along with a set of control
variables (Zk ):
H= ˇ0 + ˇ1 S +
K
ˇk Zk + error
(1)
k=2
ˆ 1i be the effect estimated by study i, and let there
Let ˇ
be a total of M estimates produced by multiple studies.2
ˆ 1i , become the
In meta-analysis, the estimated effects, ˇ
dependent variable. OLS estimation of ˛0 in the equation
2
A complication arises because increases in the health variable can
mean an improvement or a decline in health, depending on how it is
measured. For example, a positive estimate of ˇ1 in Eq. (1) when health
is measured by mortality means something different than when health
is measured by a categorical variable increasing in good health. Likewise,
sometimes a measure of social capital is defined so that a larger number
means an increase in social capital, but sometimes it is measured so that
a larger number means a decrease in social capital. In order to get the
sign of the effect consistent across studies, we standardized the signs of
the estimates so that a positive estimate implied that an increase in social
capital was associated with an increase in good health.
3
below produces a value equivalent to the arithmetic average of the M estimates.
ˆ 1i = ˛0 + εi , i = 1, 2, . . ., M
ˇ
(2)
If the individual estimates constitute a representative
sample from the population of estimated effects, then OLS
will produce an unbiased estimate of the mean true effect
of social capital on health.3
However, the OLS estimate will not be efficient. OLS
gives equal weight to the individual estimated effects.
ˆ s are estimated more precisely
But some of the ˇ
1i
than others,
as
indicated by their different standard
ˆ 1i ≡ SE . An efficient estimator would assign
errors, s.e. ˇ
i
greater weight to more precise estimates. If all estimates
come from a population with the same true effect, so that
the variation in εi is proportionate to the sampling error
– i.e., var (εi ) = (SE i )2 2 – then Weighted Least Squares
(WLS) will be efficient, with the appropriate weight being
the inverse of (SE i )2 .
This model of effect size heterogeneity is known in the
meta-analysis literature as “Fixed Effects”, and is not to be
confused with the panel data estimator of the same name.
“Fixed Effects” WLS estimation of Eq. (2) is equivalent to
dividing each observation by SE i and then estimating with
OLS:
1 ε
ˆ 1i
ˇ
= ˛0
+ i , i = 1, 2, . . ., M.
SE i
SE i
SE i
(3)
Assuming representative sampling, “Fixed Effects” WLS
estimation of ˛0 will produce an unbiased and efficient
estimate of the mean true effect of social capital on health.
Many researchers find the “Fixed Effects” model of effect
heterogeneity too restrictive. More likely, there is not a single, true effect of social capital on health, but a distribution
of true effects. This model of effect heterogeneity is known
in the meta-analysis literature as “Random Effects”, which
again should not be confused with the panel data estimator
of the same name.
Let 2 represent the variation in εi due to the fact that
estimated effects are drawn from populations with differing true effects. Assuming the twosources of variation in
εi are independent, then var (εi ) = (SE i )2 + 2 2 = ωi 2 .
The corresponding “Random Effects” WLS estimator is
equivalent to estimating the following equation by OLS:
1 ε
ˆ 1i
ˇ
= ˛0
+ i , i = 1, 2, . . ., M.
ωi
ωi
ωi
(4)
Note that the “Random Effects” estimator produces a
more uniform distribution of weights than “Fixed Effects”,
since the weighting terms include a common constant,
2 . While researchers generally agree that the “Random
Effects” model most closely matches reality, there is some
debate about which works best in practice (Doucouliagos
and Paldam, 2013; Reed, 2015). Accordingly, our analysis
uses both.
A related issue concerns the weighting of estimates
versus studies. The number of estimates per study can vary
3
We address issues of publication bias and endogeneity below.
4
X. Xue, W.R. Reed and A. Menclova / Journal of Health Economics 72 (2020) 102317
widely. In our sample, partly because many specifications
contain multiple measures of social capital, the number of
estimates per study ranges from 1 to 240, with a mean of
27. The preceding WLS estimators implicitly give greater
weight, sometimes dramatically greater weight, to studies
with more estimates. Accordingly, we employ an alternative weighting system that, ceteris paribus, gives equal
weight to studies rather than individual estimates. In the
subsequent discussion, Weight1 and Weight2 denote the
weighting systems that give equal weights to individual
estimates and studies, respectively.
Two more issues need to be addressed. The preceding assumes that it is meaningful to average estimated
effects across studies. However, in the empirical literature
on social capital and health, different measures are used for
both health and social capital. Further, different estimation
procedures are employed – linear regression, ordered probit models, hazard models, odds ratio models and more – so
that the interpretation of coefficients varies greatly, despite
the fact that the respective studies are all concerned with
estimating the effect of social capital on health.
This is a common situation in meta-analysis, and there
is a common solution: to convert the respective estimates
to partial correlation coefficients (PCCs):
PCC i =
ti
ti2
(5)
+ df i
where ti and df i are the t-statistic and degrees of freedom associated with the respective estimated effect. The
associated standard error is given by:
SE(PCC i ) =
1 − PCC 2i
df i
(6)
ˆ 1i is
The preceding analysis still holds, except that ˇ
replaced by PCC i , and SE i now stands for the standard error
of PCC i , so that Eqs (3) and (4) become
PCC i
= ˛0 ·
SE i
and
PCC i
= ˛0
ωi
1
SE i
1
ωi
+
+
εi
, i = 1, 2, . . ., M
SE i
εi
, i = 1, 2, . . ., M.
ωi
(3’)
(4’)
Accordingly, the parameter ˛0 represents the mean true
effect of social capital on health measured as a correlation.
How should one assess the estimates of ˛0 ? Like any
correlation, PCC takes values between -1 and 1. Cohen
(1988) suggested that correlation values of 0.10, 0.30, and
0.50 in absolute value should be interpreted as “small”,
“medium” and “large” effects, and his interpretation is
widely accepted. However, as Doucouliagos (2011) points
out, Cohen’s taxonomy refers to simple, not partial, correlations. To investigate partial correlation sizes, Doucouliagos
collected over 22,000 estimates in empirical economics
and converted them to PCCs. He then ranked them from
smallest to largest in absolute value. He defined the 25th,
50th, and 75th percentile values as “small”, “medium”,
and “large”. While there was some difference across subfields of economics, overall PCC values of 0.07, 0.17, and
0.33 corresponded to “small”, “medium” and “large” effect
sizes, respectively. This establishes a scale for comparing
PCC values to other PCC values in the literature, and it is
the standard we will employ in interpreting our empirical
work.4
4. Description of data
4.1. Selection of studies
We followed the MAER-Net protocols outlined by
Stanley et al. (2013) in our search for studies. To account
for the multi-dimensional nature of both social capital
and health, the following combination of key words was
used: “social capital”, “social trust”, “social networks”,
“social participation”, “social support”, “social engagement”, “social integration”, “social relationships”, “social
ties”, “reciprocity”, “social cohesion”, “social connections”,
“social connectedness”, “volunteering”, “health”, “mortality”, “depression” and “disease”.5
We followed a stepwise procedure to finalize a set of
studies from which to draw our estimated effects. First,
we excluded theoretical studies that did not report estimated effects. Second, we excluded studies that did not
report sufficient information to calculate PCC values and
their standard errors. Specifically, a study had to report a
numerical estimate for the effect of social capital on health,
and a corresponding standard error, t-statistic, confidence
interval, or p-value.
Third, we excluded studies that estimated the effect
of social capital on well-being, welfare, quality of life,
and life satisfaction. While indirectly related to health,
these outcomes are not comparable to direct health effects.
Fourth, we eliminated studies using interaction terms
and quadratic specifications of the social capital variable
4
A remaining issue concerns the determination of the t-statistic used in
calculating PCC in Eq. (5). In many cases, studies either directly report the
t-statistic corresponding to the estimated coefficient, or they report the
standard error, from which the t-statistic is easily calculated. However,
in the social capital and health literature, the most common estimation
procedure is some variant of an odds ratio model, where the estimated
coefficient is the odds ratio associated with a binary outcome. In addition
to the estimate, studies either report the standard error of the estimated odds ratio, or the lower and upper bounds of a 95% confidence
interval. In the former case, the t (or better, z) statistic is calculated by
ˆ 1i ) · ˇ
ˆ 1i /s.e.(ˇ
ˆ 1i ). In the latter case, one first calculates SE ORi =
ti = ln(ˇ
(ln(UpperBoundi ) − ln(LowerBoundi ))/(2 · 1.96), and then calculates ti =
ˆ 1i /SE ORi . Calculation of PCC proceeds accordingly. A related comln ˇ
plication arises when studies only report the coefficient and a set of stars
to indicate the level of statistical significance: e.g., *** = significant at the 1
percent level, ** = significant at the 5 percent level, * = significant at the 10
percent level, and no stars = insignificant at the 10 percent level. In these
cases, we set the p-value = 0.005, 0.025, 0.075, and 0.50, respectively, and
work backwards from the inverse of the t-distribution to calculate a tvalue. We record how we calculate our t-values to see if the respective
methods affect our results.
5
The search was conducted using the search engines EconLit, JSTOR,
EBSCO, Google Scholar, RePEc, SSRN, Social Science Citation Index (SSCI),
Science Citation Index (SCI) and Scopus. Backward and forward citation
searching was employed to leverage articles identified through the search
engine process. We also manually searched academic journals that were
found to have published studies on social capital and health. To be as
comprehensive as possible, we also searched working papers, books, doctoral dissertations, master theses, and government reports. The search
was ended in September 2017.
X. Xue, W.R. Reed and A. Menclova / Journal of Health Economics 72 (2020) 102317
because of the difficulty of combining multiple coefficients
to calculate a single estimated effect with standard error
(cf. Gunby et al., 2017).6 For similar reasons, we dropped
studies employing path analysis that use social capital as a
mediator variable. Lastly, we excluded studies measuring
the intergenerational effect of social capital on health; e.g.,
the effect of parent’s social capital on child’s health.
4.2. Study coding
For each estimate in our sample, we coded data to enable
construction of effect sizes and their standard errors, and
to record corresponding study, data and estimation characteristics. Information included the study’s author(s), type of
publication (e.g., journal), journal name, publication year,
countries included in the study sample, number of observations, and data type (cross-sectional or panel). We recorded
the estimated coefficient and associated statistics (such as
standard errors, confidence intervals, etc.).7
We categorized the estimation procedures used to produce each estimate. Binary variables were used to indicate
the following methods: OLS, FGLS, probit/logit, ordered
probit/logit, Hierarchical Linear Model (HLM), and instrumental variables (IV). In addition, a set of dummy variables
were used to indicate whether common control variables
were included in the regression specifications (e.g., age,
gender, income, etc.).
Endogeneity is certainly a concern in the social capital
and health literature. While most studies assume that the
effect runs from social capital to health, the literature does
recognize that causation can go in the opposite direction.
For this reason, our analysis will pay particular attention
to estimates derived from panel data or IV estimation procedures. Holding other factors constant, we will want to
investigate whether these estimates differ systematically
from estimates that do not address endogeneity.
For each estimate, we coded the associated health outcome, type of social capital, and whether the social capital
variable was individual-based or was aggregated at the
community level. Table 1 provides some detail about the
different measures of health employed by the studies in our
sample. Measures of health consisted of measures of physical health, mental health, and measures of overall/general
health. Among measures of physical health, the most common measure was mortality, usually “all-cause” mortality,
but sometimes mortality due to a particular illness or disease, such as cancer or cardio-vascular disease. Studies
measured mortality over given sample periods, and as the
sample periods differed in length, the interpretation of
mortality rates differed accordingly.
6
Note that we do not encounter problems with calculating marginal
effects for nonlinear models because all we require for PCC is to be able to
calculate a t-statistic. The problem arises when more than one coefficient
is involved, such as when there is an interaction or quadratic term.
7
We note that due to poor reporting practices and the generally large
number of observations used in the respective studies – the median number of observations for a given estimate in our study was approximately
3,500 – we substituted the number of observations for degrees of freedom
in calculating PCC and SE(PCC) in Eqs. (5) and (6).
5
Table 1
Common Measures of Health.
Measure
Percent
PHYSICAL HEALTH (4770 observations)
Mortality
Disease/Illness
Self-Reported
51.2
17.4
20.9
MENTAL HEALTH (2934 observations)
Depression
Self-Reported
28.2
90.9
NOTE: Authors’ calculations. Percent values are either percent of total
Physical Health observations or percent of total Mental Health observations.
Table 2
Common Types of Social Capital.
Measure
Percent
COGNITIVE / STRUCTURAL (11,776 observations)
Cognitive (What people feel)
Social trust
Perceived social support
Perceived social cohension
Perceived reciprocity
Sense of belonging
Loneliness
13.2
7.3
2.0
1.5
1.5
1.0
Structural (What people do)
Social participation
Social networks
Social support
Social engagement
Volunteering
Group membership
Social integration
Social relationship
16.7
16.4
6.7
6.5
4.8
4.6
3.9
3.5
BONDING / BRIDGING / LINKING (3572 observations)
Bonding (Horizontal ties between similar people)
Bridging (Horizontal ties between dissimilar people)
Linking (Vertical ties between different people)
62.0
34.2
3.9
NOTE: Authors’ calculations. Percent values are either percent of
total Cognitive/Structural observations or percent of total Bonding/Bridging/Linking observations.
The second most common type of physical health measure was the presence or onset of a particular illness
or disease (e.g., hypertension, heart disease, diabetes).
Various measures of overall health were also common.
Sometimes these were indices constructed from multiple
questions about a person’s health, and sometimes they
were categorical measures in which a respondent’s health
was characterized as good, fair, poor, etc. A substantial
number of the physical health measures relied on respondents’ own assessments.
Among mental health measures, depression was the
most common category of mental health. Other categories
included dementia, mental distress or anxiety, and measures of cognitive ability. As with physical health, many of
the studies used an overall measure of mental well-being.
Perhaps not surprisingly, a very large number of mental
health measures relied on self-reported assessments.
Table 2 gives a sense of the wide variety of social
capital variables used by the studies in our sample. The
most common framework employed by studies was cognitive/structural, where cognitive refers to what people feel,
and structural to what people do. Social trust was the most
6
X. Xue, W.R. Reed and A. Menclova / Journal of Health Economics 72 (2020) 102317
common type of cognitive social capital, followed by the
perception of social support, and then the perception of
social cohesion. The key element here is the respondent’s
perception of these constructs. The most common measures of structural social capital was participation in some
form of social activity, followed by measures of one’s network of personal relationships. It is noteworthy that some
measures of social capital mixed the two types of social
capital, often by composing an index of social capital that
relies on both.
An alternative framework for categorizing social capital is bonding/bridging/linking. While less common than
cognitive/structural, it is still widely used. Bonding refers
to horizontal ties between similar people, while bridging
refers to horizontal ties between dissimilar people. Linking refers to hierarchical relationships. Of these, bonding
was the most commonly used social capital variable in the
studies included in our sample.
5. Data Analysis: part 1
5.1. Descriptive Analysis
Our initial dataset consisted of 13,040 observations
gleaned from 471 studies. Calculation of PCC uses t-values
and df, so the first two columns of Table 3 focus on these
variables. The full sample of t-values has mean and median
values of 1.67 and 1.20, respectively. We will comment on
these relatively low values later. For now, we wish to note
the minimum and maximum values of -59.67 and 850. This
raises concern with outliers. A similar concern applies to
the df variable. It has mean and median values of 29,023
and 3300, with minimum and maximum values of 5 and
2,442,948.
The corresponding distribution of PCC values ranges
from -0.747 to 0.998, with mean and median values of 0.030
and 0.019. Large (absolute) values of PCC are potentially a
problem because of the key role that PCC plays in determining the standard error, and hence, the weights used in
the empirical analysis: s.e.(PCC i ) =
1−PCC 2
i
, with weights
df i
increasing in the absolute value of PCC.
As a result, we proceed by truncating the top and bottom 1% of PCC values, leaving 12,778 observations from
470 studies. The truncated distributions of t-statistic, df,
and PCC values are also reported in Table 3, immediately
to the right of the full sample statistics.8 Corresponding
histograms for the t-statistics and PCC values are reported
in Fig. 1. The two histograms in Fig. 1 and corresponding
columns in Table 3 go far in answering our first question
about the size of the effect of social capital on health.
The mean and median PCC values for the truncated sample are 0.028 and 0.019. Based on Doucouliagos (2011),
these do not even get close to the threshold value of 0.07
that Doucouliagos sets for “small”. If social capital has an
8
As a robustness check, we also truncated the observations based on
the lowest and highest one-percent of t-values and df’s. We redid all the
analyses with this alternative truncation strategy. The results were very
similar to those reported in this paper, and the qualitative conclusions
were identical.
Fig. 1. Distribution of t and PCC Values.
effect on health, these values suggest that the effect is very
small. The reasons for the small PCC values are not hard to
identify. First, a large number of estimates in the literature
are statistically insignificant. The table immediately below
the histogram in the top panel of Fig. 1 reports that 56.4% of
all t-values lie between -2 and 2. Compounding these relatively low t-values are relatively large sample sizes. The
distribution of df values for the truncated sample ranges
from 5 to 2,442,948, with a median value of 3,451. If we
calculate the PCC value that corresponds to the median t
and df values using Eq. (5), we obtain a value for PCC equal
to 0.020.
However, there are two caveats. First, the numbers
in Table 3, and the values represented in Fig. 1, are
unweighted. So we need to re-compute our estimate of the
mean true effect, ˛0 , using the different weighting schemes
described above. Second, the analysis ignores publication
bias.
Publication bias arises when the results reported by
researchers, and/or the studies accepted for publication
by journals, comprise a biased sample of the population
of all estimates. Note that “publication bias” can occur
X. Xue, W.R. Reed and A. Menclova / Journal of Health Economics 72 (2020) 102317
7
Table 3
Descriptive Statistics for Estimated Effects and t-statistics.
t-Statistics
Mean
Median
Minimum
Maximum
Std. Dev.
1%
5%
10%
90%
95%
99%
Obs
df
PCC Values
Full
Truncated
Full
Truncated
Full
Truncated
1.67
1.20
−59.67
850.00
8.40
−4.91
−2.66
−1.75
4.70
7.16
15.34
13,040
1.60
1.20
−17.44
48.86
3.53
−4.37
−2.58
−1.63
4.67
7.02
14.60
12,778
29,023
3300
5
2,442,948
201,648
39
190
412
23,153
44,986
271,642
13,040
29,600
3451
5
2,442,948
203,663
49
244
462
23,153
44,986
1,358,932
12,778
0.030
0.019
−0.747
0.998
0.084
−0.157
−0.061
−0.033
0.102
0.143
0.361
13,040
0.028
0.019
−0.156
0.360
0.059
−0.113
−0.055
−0.031
0.097
0.132
0.239
12,778
NOTE: The truncated sample is obtained from the Full Sample by deleting observations having the top and bottom 1% of PCC values.
even in working papers that are not published in journals. This can happen if researchers choose not to write up
results because the initial analyses did not produce interesting/promising findings.9 In that case, even unpublished
working papers can be characterized by publication bias.
Publication bias is widely recognized as a problem, with
selection typically favoring estimates that are statistically
significant, and/or consistent with researchers’ and journals’ preconceived beliefs (Christensen and Miguel, 2018).
That being said, we note that in order for publication bias to
explain the low PCC values we see in our sample, it would
have to discriminate against statistically significant estimates. Most researchers would view this as unlikely.
Fig. 1 and Table 3 present “overall” estimates, aggregating estimates of the effects of social capital across different
kinds of health outcomes. To address concerns about the
validity of combining these estimates, Fig. 2 breaks the full
sample of PCC values into three subsamples, depending
on whether the health outcome is physical health, mental
health, or general health. A table at the bottom of the figure
reports means and standard deviations for each of the three
subsamples. Mean PCC values are similar, with most values lying within the range characterized by Doucouliagos
(2011) as small. The similar distributions provide casual
support for the legitimacy of combining estimates for the
different health outcomes. We will explore this subject in
greater detail below.
Table 4 describes some of the other variables in our
data. Approximately 98% of the estimates in our sample are
drawn from peer-reviewed journals (Journal). It is common
in meta-analyses to include a mix of published and unpublished studies, mostly to address publication bias. While
our initial search produced a larger set of working papers,
most of these were subsequently published, and were eliminated as duplicates. As it turns out, publication bias ends
up not being a major concern in our study. While we find
evidence of publication bias in our subsequent analysis, its
estimated influence is very small, a result foreshadowed by
the small PCC values reported above. The variable PubYear
9
Franco et al. (2014) report that the main source of publication bias
is failure of researchers to write up results that are not significant or
interesting.
reports that the studies in our sample were published in
the window from 1985 to 2017.
Social capital can be constructed on an individuallevel (Glaeser et al., 2002) or group-level (Coleman, 1990;
Putnam, 1995) basis. 86.5% of the estimates in our sample
are associated with individual-level measures (IndividualSC). Our analysis will address whether individual- and
group-level measures of social capital have different effects
on health.
Our sample covers a diverse set of countries, with the
largest number of estimates using data from Western or
Northern Europe (33.9%). East Asia (20.4%) and the USA
(20.9%) were also common areas to study, but a substantial
number of estimates came from countries other than those
above.
Of the three categories of health outcomes, most studies
examined the effect of social capital on a general measure
of health (40.4% of estimates), closely followed by physical health (37.3%) and then mental health (23.0%). Two
thirds of the estimated effects were based on self-reported
assessments of health.
Almost all of the estimates were derived using social
capital variables categorized as cognitive and structural
social capital (92.2%). Another perspective, which includes
bonding, bridging, and linking social capital, was less commonly employed (28.0%). It is quite common for studies
to include an array of social capital variables in a single
regression equation (NumberSCVariables). In our sample,
these ranged from 1 to 28 separate social capital variables
in a single specification, with a mean of 6.5 social capital
variables per equation.
We also tracked the control variables that were commonly included in studies of social capital and health. 85.2%
of estimates came from a regression specification including
an age variable; 84.5% included a gender variable; 59.9%
included an education variable; 38.9% included a marital
status variable; and 40.3% included an income variable.
A large number of different estimation procedures were
used to produce the estimated effects in our sample. The
most common procedure involved the odds ratio model,
such as logistic regression or hazard model estimation
(55.2%). The next two most common procedures were hierarchical linear modelling (17.9%) and ordinary least squares
8
X. Xue, W.R. Reed and A. Menclova / Journal of Health Economics 72 (2020) 102317
Table 4
Description of Variables.
Variable
Description
Mean
Min
Max
Study type
Journal
PubYear
= 1, if study is a journal article
Year study was published/appeared
0.977
2009.3
0
1985
1
2017
Data characteristics
IndividualSC
Panel
Cross-sectional*
= 1, if estimate based on individual-level social capital variable
= 1, if estimate based on panel data
= 1, if estimate based on cross-sectional data
0.865
0.444
0.556
0
0
0
1
1
1
Countries
EastAsia
USA*
WestNorthEurope
HighIncome
OtherCountry
= 1, if country studied is located in East Asia
= 1, if country studied is USA
= 1, if country studied is located in Western or Northern Europe
= 1, if country studied is a high income country not included above
= 1, if country studied is none of the above
0.204
0.209
0.339
0.094
0.155
0
0
0
0
0
1
1
1
1
1
Health measure
PhysicalHealth
MentalHealth
GeneralHealth*
SelfReported
= 1, if health variable measures physical health
= 1, if health variable measures mental health
= 1, if health variable measures overall health
= 1, if health variable is self-reported
0.373
0.230
0.404
0.679
0
0
0
0
1
1
1
1
Social capital measure
CognitiveStructural
BondBridgeLink
NumberSCVariables
= 1, if social capital is cognitive or structural
= 1, if social capital is bonding, bridging, or linking
Number of social capital variables included in the regression
0.922
0.280
6.49
0
0
1
1
1
28
Control variables
Age
Gender
Education
MaritalStatus
Income
= 1, if an age variable is included in the regression
= 1, if a gender variable is included in the regression
= 1, if an education variable is included in the regression
= 1, if a marital status variable is included in the regression
= 1, if an income variable is included in the regression
0.852
0.845
0.599
0.389
0.403
0
0
0
0
0
1
1
1
1
1
Estimation method
OLS
ORHazard
HLM
FGLS*
ProbitLogit*
OrderedProbitLogit*
IV*
OtherEstimation*
SENonspherical
= 1, if estimation method is OLS
= 1, if estimation method is Odds Ratio or Hazards Ratio
= 1, if estimation method is Hierarchical Linear Modelling
= 1, if estimation method is FGLS
= 1, if estimation method is Probit or Logit
= 1, if estimation method is Ordered Probit or Logit
= 1, if estimation method is Instrumental Variables
= 1, if estimation method is none of the above
= 1, if standard error estimation assumes nonspherical errors
0.139
0.552
0.179
0.017
0.053
0.031
0.019
0.010
0.255
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
Calculation of t-statistic
tNormal
tCalculatedBypValue
tCalculatedByCI*
= 1, if t-statistic is calculated as ratio of coefficient to standard error
= 1, if t-statistic is calculated from p-value
= 1, if t-statistic is calculated from confidence interval
0.215
0.178
0.606
0
0
0
1
1
1
NOTE: When the grouped variables include all possible categories, the categories omitted in the subsequent analysis (the benchmark categories) are
indicated by an asterisk.
(13.9%). Other procedures included probit and logit models, ordered probit and logit models, feasible generalized
least squares, and instrumental variable estimation. We
note that only a very low percentage of estimates employed
instrumental variable procedures (1.9%).
The standard error of the estimated effect plays an
important role in weighting observations. As a result,
we recorded the procedure used to estimate the standard error, categorizing these as either assuming spherical
errors (homoskedasticity, error independence) or not.
25.5% of the estimates calculated standard errors assuming
some form of nonspherical error behavior.
Finally, three different methods were used to derive
t-values. In 21.5% of the cases, t-statistics were either
directly reported, or could be calculated by dividing the
estimated coefficient by its reported standard error (tNor-
mal). In 17.8% of the cases, all we had was a p-value, and
we worked backwards from the inverse t-distribution function to obtain a t-value (tCalculatedBypValue). However, in
most cases (60.6%), the t-statistic was calculated using the
reported confidence interval or from the log of the estimated odds ratio (tCalculatedByCI).10
While we are aware of no study that compares the frequency of “Fixed Effects” and “Random Effects” estimators
in the meta-analysis literature, our sense is that “Fixed
Effects” is generally preferred by researchers. Table 5 identifies a concern with “Fixed Effects”. It calculates a “study
weight” for each study in our sample, weighting the individual estimates of that study by the respective weighting
10
See footnote #4 for more details.
X. Xue, W.R. Reed and A. Menclova / Journal of Health Economics 72 (2020) 102317
9
Table 5
Study Weights.
Mean
Median
1%
5%
10%
90%
95%
99%
Maximum
Top 3
Top 10
Studies
Fixed Effects
Random Effects
0.21%
0.01%
0.0001%
0.0003%
0.0007%
0.1622%
0.3251%
1.9023%
45.2%
67.4%
77.3%
470
0.21%
0.23%
0.0467%
0.0959%
0.1399%
0.2576%
0.2607%
0.2636%
0.26%
0.79%
2.63%
470
NOTE: The methodology for calculating “study weights” is described in
Footnote #11 in the text.
45.2%.12 The top 3 studies (out of 470!) account for 67.4%
of the total weight, and the top 10 studies comprise almost
77.3%. Thus the “Fixed Effects” estimate will disproportionately weight a small number of select studies that have
large PCC values and/or use a large number of observations
(df).
In contrast, the large size of 2 in the “Random Effects”
estimator swamps the size of the individual (SE i )2 terms,
so that the “Random Effects” estimator weights the individual estimates much more uniformly.13 The median value is
0.23%, compared to a mean value of 0.21%. The maximum
weight any single study receives is 0.26%, and the top 10
studies account for 2.63%. This arguably overcompensates
the extreme skewness of the “Fixed Effects” estimator. In
what follows, we report both “Fixed Effects” and “Random
Effects”, with a mild preference for the latter because it is
not so heavily dependent on a small number of studies. As
a practical matter, all of our key findings are robust across
estimators.
5.2. Publication bias
Fig. 2. Distribution of PCC Values by Measures of Health.
scheme (“Fixed Effects”/“Random Effects”) and then aggregating the weights at the study level. In this way, each study
receives a weight, the sum of which equals 100%.11
If the 100% weight was divided equally across studies,
given 470 studies, each study would receive a weight of
0.21%. Against this benchmark, “Fixed Effects” produces a
highly skewed weighting distribution. The median weight
is 0.01%, and the maximum weight for a single study is
11
Study weights were calculated by wi /
or wi = 1/ (SE i )2 + 2
2
wi , where wi = 1/(SE i )
depending on whether Fixed Effects or Random
Effects were being used (cf. Rinquist, 2013, page 128).
Publication bias represents a serious challenge to the
validity of meta-analysis. If the estimates in the literature
are disproportionately large and significant, then averaging
them will preserve this bias, producing a distorted estimate
of the mean true effect. Methods to identify and correct
publication bias remains an active research area in the
meta-analysis literature (Andrews and Kasy, 2017; Stanley
et al., 2017; Alinaghi and Reed, 2018).
A common test for publication bias is given by the
Funnel Asymmetry Test (FAT). The FAT is carried out by
adding the standard error variable, SE, to the constantonly regressions above. It is designed to capture the idea
that publication bias introduces a systematic relationship
between the effect size (PCC) and its standard error (SE):
PCC i = ˛0 + ˛1 SE i + εi
(7)
12
Our ID for this study is 177 (Blakely et al., 2006). It has 72 estimated
effects of social capital on health, and the number of observations in the
respective samples ranges from 2,306,760 to 2,442,948.
13
The corresponding I2 value is 89.9%, which indicates that 2 is approximately nine times as large as the average SE 2 value.
10
X. Xue, W.R. Reed and A. Menclova / Journal of Health Economics 72 (2020) 102317
The FAT consists of testing the significance of SE. Rejection of H0 : ˛1 = 0 is taken as evidence that publication bias
exists. If we divide Eq. (7) by the weights SE and ω, we
obtain:
PCC i
= ˛0
SE i
1
SE i
ε
+˛1 + i .
SE i
(3”)
and
PCC i
= ˛0 ·
ωi
1
ωi
+ ˛1 ·
SE
i
ωi
+
εi
ωi
(4”)
6. Data Analysis: part 2
6.1. Meta-regression analysis
The preceding section has been concerned with estimating the mean true effect of social capital on health. In this
section, we investigate factors that affect the size of that
effect. To do that, we add potential moderator variables
Xk , k = 1, 2, . . .K, into the specification of Eq. (7):
PCC i = ˛0 + ˛1 SE i +
K
˛k+1 Xki + εi
(8)
k=1
It is standard practice when the meta-analysis sample consists of multiple estimates from the same study to
correct for non-independence of the error terms by using
cluster robust standard errors.
Inclusion of the SE variable also serves to control for the
influence of publication bias on the estimate of mean true
effect, ˛0 . Thus, the estimate of ˛0 in the specification of Eq.
(7) represents the bias-adjusted estimate of the mean true
effect of social capital on health. A test of the significance of
˛
ˆ 0 is known as the Precision Effect Test (PET). Rejection of
H0 : ˛0 = 0 is taken as evidence that the mean true effect
of social capital on health is nonzero.
Table 6 reports the FAT/PET results, with the FAT results
reported on the first row and the PET results on the second.
The first four columns report the various combinations of
“Fixed Effects”/“Random Effects” and weighting by individual estimate(“Weight1”)/weighting by study(“Weight2”).
Across all four columns, we reject H0 : ˛1 = 0 at the 1
percent level of significance, indicating the existence of
publication bias. The positive coefficient indicates positive
publication bias, suggesting sample selection that favors
the publication of positive estimates of the effect of social
capital on health.
In three of the four cases we also reject H0 : ˛0 = 0, with
the corresponding estimates of ˛0 significant at the 1 percent level. The exception is the “Fixed Effects(Weight1)”
regression. Thus, the PET results generally support the finding that social capital is positively and significantly related
to health. However, the sizes of the coefficient estimates
indicate that this effect is very small. Bias-adjusted estimates of the mean true effect of social capital on health
range from 0.004 to 0.022, substantially below the value
that Doucouliagos (2011) identifies as being “small”.
5.3. Meta-regression analysis
Columns (5) and (6) in Table 6 report the weighted
average estimates of mean true effect, uncorrected for
publication bias, using the “Random Effects(Weight1)”
and “Random Effects(Weight2)” estimators. The associated estimates are 0.024 and 0.032, which are close to the
unweighted value of 0.028 reported in Table 3. These fall
to 0.013 and 0.022, respectively, when SE is added to the
specification to control for publication bias. Thus, while
publication bias positively inflates estimates of the effect
of social capital on health, it does not inflate them very
much.
The coefficient ˛k+1 measures the change in the effect
of social capital on health due to Xk , where a positive
coefficient indicates that studies/regressions that have
characteristic Xk estimate a larger effect of social capital
on health. The specification of Eq. (8) is known as a metaregression.
Given the large number of study, data, and estimation
characteristics included in our dataset (cf. Table 4), we
are concerned that multicollinearity may disguise significant relationships. Accordingly, we adopt a model selection
algorithm to select a “best” specification. We use a backwards stepwise procedure that is designed to select the
model specification with the smallest Bayesian Information Criterion value (Lindsey and Sheather, 2010). In the
first round, all the variables are included in the regression
equation: SE, PubYear, IndividualSC, Panel, EastAsia, WestNorthEurope, HighIncome, OtherCountry, PhysicalHealth,
MentalHealth, SelfReported, NumberSCVariables, Age, Gender,
Education, MaritalStatus, Income, OLS, ORHazard, HLM, IV,
SENonspherical, tNormal, and tCalculatedbypValue. At each
subsequent round, the algorithm drops the variable that
causes the largest decrease in BIC. It continues to do that,
one variable at a time, until the BIC can no longer be
reduced. We then re-estimate the final, best model in order
to obtain cluster robust standard errors.14
We do this for each of the four estimation procedures
(“Fixed Effects(Weight1)”, “Fixed Effects(Weight2)”, “RandomEffects(Weight1)”, and “RandomEffects(Weight2)”.
We forced five variables to be retained in each step of
the selection process: two variables to indicate the type
of health outcome, PhysicalHealth and MentalHealth, with
the omitted category being general health; a variable
indicating that the respective social capital variable was
individual-level as opposed to a community average (IndividualSC); and two variables that represent attempts to deal
with endogeneity, Panel and IV.
14
We encountered a problem when using the stepwise regression algorithm with the “Random Effects” estimators. Note that there is no constant
term in the weighted specification of equation (4”), as the constant term
in the original equation is divided by ωi . The Stata program that we used,
vselect, does not allow one to drop the constant term. Our workaround
was to estimate the “best” model with a constant term, and then estimate
that same variable specification, but without the constant term. Note that
was not a problem for the “Fixed Effects” estimators, because the publication bias variable, SE, reduces to the constant term when divided through
by SE (see equation 3”). In this case, the “constant” term is actually the
coefficient on SE, ˛1 .
X. Xue, W.R. Reed and A. Menclova / Journal of Health Economics 72 (2020) 102317
11
Table 6
The Funnel Asymmetry Test (FAT) and Precision Effect Test (PET).
Including Publication Bias Term
FAT
PET
Observations
Excluding Publication Bias Term
Fixed Effects
(Weight1) (1)
Fixed Effects
(Weight2) (2)
Random Effects
(Weight1) (3)
Random Effects
(Weight2) (4)
Random Effects
(Weight1) (5)
Random Effects
(Weight2) (6)
1.269*** (5.93)
0.004 (1.45)
12,778
1.491*** (6.98)
0.007*** (2.87)
12,778
0.599*** (5.33)
0.013*** (5.15)
12,778
0.551*** (4.34)
0.022*** (7.49)
12,778
---0.024*** (12.79)
12,778
---0.032*** (16.17)
12,778
NOTE: The FAT and PET results in Columns (1) through (4) come from estimating ˛1 and ˛0 , respectively, in Eq. (7) in the text using Weighted Least Squares
(WLS). The four WLS estimators (Fixed Effects-Weight1, Fixed Effects-Weight2, Random Effects-Weight1, and Random Effects-Weight2) are described in Section
2 of the text. The PET results in Columns (5) and (6) are taken from estimates of Eq. (4’). All of the estimation procedures calculate cluster robust standard
errors. *, **, and *** indicate statistical significance at the 10-, 5-, and 1-percent level, respectively.
Table 7
Meta-Regression Analysis.
Variables
Fixed Effects
(Weight1) (1)
Fixed Effects
(Weight2) (2)
Random Effects
(Weight1) (3)
Random Effects
(Weight2) (4)
SE
PhysicalHealth
MentalHealth
IndividualSC
Panel
IV
PubYear
EastAsia
WestNorthEurope
HighIncome
OtherCountry
SelfReported
NumberSCVariables
Age
Education
MaritalStatus
OLS
HLM
SENonspherical
tNormal
tCalculatedBypValue
Adjusted R-squared
Hypothesis Test: Physical = Mental = 0
Observations
0.468*** (3.22)
−0.008** (−2.37)
−0.006* (−1.95)
0.013*** (4.84)
−0.006** (−2.41)
−0.000 (−0.06)
---0.006 (1.60)
0.009*** (2.91)
0.003 (0.78)
---0.007** (2.18)
−0.001*** (−3.92)
−0.003* (−1.96)
−0.002* (−1.74)
−0.003 (−1.36)
0.006 (1.02)
---−0.003 (−1.10)
−0.010*** (−3.47)
−0.023*** (−7.72)
0.206
F = 3.15 (p = 0.044)
12,778
0.641*** (4.77)
−0.003* (−0.59)
−0.004 (−0.99)
0.020*** (5.50)
−0.006** (−2.23)
0.002 (0.50)
−0.000 (-1.15)
0.006* (1.69)
0.010*** (3.07)
0.006 (1.44)
---0.015*** (2.97)
−0.002*** (−4.73)
---−0.004* (−1.92)
−0.005** (−2.08)
0.006 (0.86)
0.006 (1.51)
−0.007 (−1.66)
−0.011** (−3.61)
−0.027*** (−6.93)
0.226
F = 0.53 (p = 0.59)
12,778
—
−0.012*** (-3.61)
−0.006* (-1.93)
0.015*** (3.62)
−0.014*** (−4.52)
−0.007 (−1.38)
------0.009*** (2.66)
---−0.005 (-1.24)
---−0.001*** (−3.93)
−0.004 (−1.06)
−0.006** (−2.09)
------—
−0.007** (−2.11)
---−0.023*** (−6.39)
0.275
F = 7.13 (p = 0.001)
12,778
—
−0.006 (−0.90)
−0.003 (−0.72)
0.018*** (4.52)
−0.012*** (−2.85)
−0.000 (−0.04)
−0.000 (−1.37)
0.006 (1.12)
0.007 (1.59)
0.006 (0.81)
---0.010 (1.61)
−0.002*** (−5.38)
---−0.010** (−2.44)
−0.004 (−1.11)
0.007 (0.98)
—
−0.006 (−1.46)
−0.007 (−1.61)
−0.034*** (−6.58)
0.302
F = 0.48 (p = 0.621)
12,778
NOTE: The table reports the results of estimating Eq. (8) in the text. The top value in each cell is the coefficient estimate, and the bottom value in parentheses
is the associated t-statistic. The variables PhysicalHealth, MentalHealth, IndividualSC, Panel, and IV were locked into each regression specification. Remaining
control variables were selected using a backwards stepwise regression procedure that chooses variables to minimize the Bayes Information Criterion. The
four WLS estimators (Fixed Effects-Weight1, Fixed Effects-Weight2, Random Effects-Weight1, and Random Effects-Weight2) are described in Section 2. All four
estimation procedures calculate cluster robust standard errors. *, **, and *** indicate statistical significance at the 10-, 5-, and 1-percent level, respectively.
The hypothesis test reports the results of testing whether there is no difference in mean PCC values for the three different health outcomes after controlling
for the effects of other variables.
The results are reported in Table 7. The publication bias
term SE is positive and statistically significant at the 1
percent level in two of the four estimation procedures.15
However, the estimated coefficients suggest that the size of
the bias is small.16 Thus, while the social capital and health
literature may be affected by publication bias, it is of little
practical consequence.
Neither of the two health outcome variables are consistently significant. PhysicalHealth is significant at the
5-percent level in two of the regressions (FixedEffects-
15
The SE variable is dropped by the selection algorithm for the Random
Effects(Weight2) regression.
16
Doucouliagos and Stanley (2013) determine that “the literature suffers
from substantial selectivity” if the estimated publication bias coefficient
has an absolute value greater than 1. Note that the SE coefficient is less
than 1 in all three of the regressions in which it appears.
Weight1, RandomEffects-Weight1). MentalHealth never
achieves significance. This confirms the casual observation
from Fig. 2 regarding the similarity of the PCC distributions
for the three different health outcomes. Nevertheless, to
be cautious, our subsequent analysis of the social capital
variables will – in addition to pooling the PCC values –
also divide the full sample into subsamples based on the
nature of the health outcomes.
In contrast, we find strong evidence that social capital variables based on individual-level measures are more
effectual for positive health outcomes than community
average measures. However, while the IndividualSC estimates are significant at the 1-percent level in all four
regressions, the sizes of the estimated differences in PCC
are small, ranging between 0.013 and 0.020.
We find weak evidence that correcting for endogeneity
diminishes the estimated effect of social capital on health.
12
X. Xue, W.R. Reed and A. Menclova / Journal of Health Economics 72 (2020) 102317
The coefficients for Panel are consistently negative across
the four regressions. While significant at the 5-percent
level in every case, the sizes of the estimated coefficients
are small. The IV estimates are even weaker – very small in
size and never attaining significance at even the 10-percent
level.
As we proceed to the other variables included in the
meta-regressions of Table 7, we limit our discussion to
those that are significant at the 5 percent level in at
least three of the four regressions. The coefficient for
WestNorthEurope is positive, indicating that social capital
is estimated to have a larger effect on health for residents of Western and Northern Europe compared to other
countries. The negative coefficient on NumberSCVariables
indicates that the estimated effect of a given social capital
variable tends to be smaller when more social capital variables are included in the regression. This is to be expected
because the estimated effect variable, PCC, is a function of
the t-statistic in the original study. One would expect that
the more social capital variables there are in the equation,
the more likely collinearity will reduce the significance of
any given social capital variable.
The last variable in our meta-regression specification
is tCalculatedBypValue. For approximately one-fifth of the
estimated effects in our sample, the only information
related to coefficient standard errors reported by the original study was stars; e.g. three stars to indicate significance
at the 1 percent level, two stars to indicate significance at
the 5 percent level, and so on. In our original coding, we
assigned p-values for each number of stars.17 When the
coefficient was insignificant and no stars were reported,
we set the p-value equal to 0.50.
The negative coefficient estimates for tCalculatedBypValue suggest that this was not a good approximation.
Estimated effects calculated from the resulting t-values
were significantly lower than those calculated following standard procedures. Further, compared to the other
effects for binary variables reported in Table 7, the coefficient sizes are relatively large in absolute value, ranging
from −0.023 to −0.034. The takeaway from this is that our
estimates of the unconditional mean true effect are downwardly biased by the inclusion of estimated effects using
these t-values. However, even after compensating for this,
the estimated mean true effect fails to reach the 0.07 value
that Doucouliagos identifies as “small”.
6.2. Examination by type of social capital
The last part of our analysis carries out a closer examination of the social capital variables included in our
sample. The top panel of Table 8 provides some statistical
detail about the different kinds of social capital variables
that studies have employed. Most studies in our sample
use social capital variables that fit within the cognitivestructural framework. Of the 12,778 estimates of the effects
of social capital on health in our sample, 11,776 use social
capital variables that can either be classified as cognitive or
17
See Footnote #4.
structural – approximately 92%. Of these, the great majority
are structural: 69.4%, versus 30.6% that are cognitive.
In contrast, only 3,572 of the estimated effects fit within
the bonding–bridging–linking framework. Indeed, many of
these can be cross-classified as either cognitive or structural. The breakdown for the bonding-bridging-linking
social capital variables are 62.0% bonding, 34.2% bridging,
and only 3.9% linking.
The bottom panel of Table 8 reports that there are 3,603
estimated effects that are based on a cognitive social capital variable; 8,173 effects are based on a structural social
capital variable; 2,213 are based on a bonding social capital variable; and so on. For each subsample, we calculate
the mean, unweighted PCC value unadjusted for publication bias. Mean PCC values for the Structural = 1, Bonding =
1, and Bridging = 1 subsamples are 0.022, 0.023, and 0.025,
respectively – very close to the mean value of 0.028 for
the entire sample. In contrast, the Cognitive = 1 and Linking
= 1 subsamples are different, with the Cognitive subsample showing a higher (0.041), and the Linking subsample a
lower (0.010) mean PCC value.
To investigate these differences further, we employ the
same meta-regression analysis we used in Table 7, only
this time we add social capital variables.18 In the first set
of exercises, we add Cognitive, so that the omitted social
capital variable is Structural. In the next set of exercises,
we add Bonding and Bridging for the full sample analysis,
with Linking being the omitted variable; and Bonding for
the subsample analysis, with Bridging and Linking being
the omitted variables19 We then implement the backwards
stepwise regression procedure described above.
Table 9 displays the results for the Cognitive/Structural
framework. In the interests of brevity and to focus attention, we only report estimated coefficients for the social
capital variables. The top panel uses the full sample of
11,776 estimates that are derived from either cognitive or structural social capital variables. Across all four
regressions, the coefficient for Cognitive is positive and statistically significant at the 1 percent level. This indicates
that, in general, estimates of social capital on health that
rely on cognitive social capital variables will find larger
effects than those that rely on structural social capital variables.
The next three panels stratify the sample by the three
health outcomes: physical health, mental health, and general health. The Cognitive coefficient is significant twice (at
at least the 5-percent level) in the general health subsample, three times in the physical health subsample, and in
all four of the regressions for the mental health subsample.
However, the estimated values are still small by Doucouliagos’ (2011) standards.
18
Another difference is that we do not force any variables into the equation other than a constant term and the respective social capital variables.
In the weighted regressions of equations (3’) and (4’), the “constant term”
is actually the slope coefficient on the
1
SE i
and
1
ωi
terms, respectively.
We forced these variables into the equation so that there would always
be a constant term in the final regression.
19
We could not add both Bonding and Bridging in the subsample analysis
because there were too few observations of Linking.
X. Xue, W.R. Reed and A. Menclova / Journal of Health Economics 72 (2020) 102317
13
Table 8
Social Capital Variables.
A. Sample Statistics for Social Capital Variables
Type of Social Capital
Obs
Mean
Min
Max
Cognitive
Structural
Bonding
Bridging
Linking
11,776
11,776
3,572
3,572
3,572
0.306
0.694
0.620
0.342
0.039
0
0
0
0
0
1
1
1
1
1
B. Distribution of PCC Values by Type of Social Capital
Type of Social Capital
Obs
Mean
Min
Max
Cognitive = 1
Structural = 1
Bonding = 1
Bridging = 1
Linking = 1
3,603
8,173
2,213
1,220
139
0.041
0.022
0.023
0.025
0.010
−0.155
−0.157
−0.157
−0.150
−0.073
0.360
0.360
0.347
0.234
0.308
NOTE: Values in the table report sample statistics of the social capital variables for two sets of observations. The first set of 11,776 observations consist of
estimates on health of social capital variables using the cognitive/structural framework. The second set of 3,572 observations consist of estimates based on
the bonding/bridging/linking framework.
NOTE: Values in the table report conditional mean, minimum, and maximum values of the PCC variable for observations satisfying the condition in the
leftmost column.
Table 9
Meta-Regression Analysis: Cognitive/Structural.
Variables
Fixed Effects (Weight1) (1)
Fixed Effects(Weight2) (2)
Random Effects(Weight1) (3)
Random Effects (Weight2) (4)
Full sample
Cognitive
Observations
0.012*** (4.54)
11,776
0.013*** (4.03)
11,776
0.011*** (3.65)
11,776
0.017*** (4.00)
11,776
Physical health = 1
Cognitive
Observations
0.012** (2.02)
4,421
0.014*** (3.91)
4,421
0.011** (2.41)
4,421
0.016* (1.96)
4,421
Mental health = 1
Cognitive
Observations
0.024*** (4.14)
2,638
0.028*** (4.01)
2,638
0.026*** (4.41)
2,638
0.028*** (3.40)
2,638
General health = 1
Cognitive
Observations
0.011*** (3.72)
4,810
0.011*** (3.42)
4,810
0.005 (1.15)
4,810
0.007 (1.60)
4,810
0.008** (2.30)
0.004 (0.45)
0.024** (2.26)
11,776
0.005 (1.18)
0.005 (0.85)
0.022*** (3.27)
11,776
0.008* (1.69)
0.009 (0.85)
0.028*** (2.91)
11,776
Full sample with interaction terms
0.010*** (3.55)
Cognitive
−0.001 (−0.17)
Cognitive*Physical
0.015** (2.22)
Cognitive*Mental
11,776
Observations
NOTE: The table reports the results of estimating Eq. (8) in the text for different samples of estimates, using different estimation procedures. Only the
coefficient estimates for the social capital variables are reported. The top value in each cell is the coefficient estimate, and the bottom value in parentheses
is the associated t-statistic. In addition to a constant term, the variable Cognitive was locked into the regressions in the first four sets of regressions (Full
Sample, Physical Health = 1, Mental Health = 1, General Health = 1). The last set of regressions (Full Sample with Interaction Terms) also locked in the interaction
terms Cognitive*Physical and Cognitive*Mental. The remaining control variables were selected using a backwards stepwise regression procedure that chooses
variables to minimize the Bayes Information Criterion. The four WLS estimators (Fixed Effects-Weight1, Fixed Effects-Weight2, Random Effects-Weight1, and
Random Effects-Weight2) are described in Section 2. All four calculate cluster robust standard errors. *, **, and *** indicate statistical significance at the 10-,
5-, and 1-percent level, respectively.
The last panel of Table 9 again uses the full sample,
but includes interaction terms for cognitive social capital and physical health (Cognitive*Physical), and cognitive
social capital and mental health (Cognitive*Mental). The
omitted health category is GeneralHealth. The coefficients
should be interpreted as follows: The coefficient on Cognitive represents the difference between the mean estimated
effect of cognitive social capital on general health, and
the mean estimated effect of structural social capital on
any kind of health. The coefficient on Cognitive*Physical
(or Cognitive*Mental) represents the difference between
the mean estimated effect of cognitive social capital on
physical health (or mental health) compared to the mean
estimated effect of structural social capital on any kind of
health.
Across the four regressions in Table 9, only the coefficient for Cognitive*Mental is consistently significant at the
5 percent level. The mean partial correlation of cognitive
social capital and health is estimated to be approximately
0.02 to 0.03 larger than the mean partial correlation of
structural social capital on any kind of health. Together
with the previous results, these estimates indicate that cog-
14
X. Xue, W.R. Reed and A. Menclova / Journal of Health Economics 72 (2020) 102317
Table 10
Meta-Regression Analysis: Bonding/Bridging/Linking.
Variables
Fixed Effects (Weight1) (1)
Fixed Effects (Weight2) (2)
Random Effects (Weight1) (3)
Random Effects (Weight2) (4)
Full sample
Bonding
Bridging
Ho : Bonding = Bridging = 0
Observations
0.002 (0.80)
0.000 (0.04)
F = 0.32 p-value = 0.724
3,572
0.002 (1.42)
−0.000 (−0.07)
F = 1.02 p-value = 0.362
3,572
0.010* (1.74)
0.011* (1.84)
F = 1.81 p-value = 0.166
3,572
0.017** (2.38)
0.016** (2.45)
F = 3.33 p-value = 0.038
3,572
Physical health = 1
Bonding
Observations
−0.001 (-0.50)
1,023
0.004 (1.43)
1,023
0.009 (1.50)
1,023
0.015** (2.31)
1,023
Mental health = 1
Bonding
Observations
0.008 (0.96)
1,004
0.003 (0.24)
1,004
0.005 (0.81)
1,004
−0.002 (−0.37)
1,004
General health = 1
Bonding
Observations
0.001 (0.10)
1,597
0.003 (0.37)
1,597
−0.005 (−1.13)
1,597
0.000 (0.08)
1,597
0.005 (0.64)
−0.003 (-0.38)
−0.009 (-0.83)
3,572
−0.006 (−1.50)
0.014* (1.79)
0.010 (1.35)
3,572
0.004 (0.56)
0.003 (0.28)
−0.007 (−0.68)
3,572
Full sample with interaction terms
Bonding
−0.001 (−0.15)
0.004 (0.47)
Bonding*Physical
0.003 (0.56)
Bonding*Mental
3,572
Observations
NOTE: The table reports the results of estimating Eq. (8) in the text for different samples of estimates, using different estimation procedures. Only the
coefficient estimates for the social capital variables are reported. The top value in each cell is the coefficient estimate, and the bottom value in parentheses
is the associated t-statistic. In addition to a constant term, the following variables were locked into the regressions: Bonding and Bridging for the set of
Full Sample regressions; Bonding for the set of Physical Health = 1, Mental Health = 1, General Health = 1 regressions; and Bonding, Bonding*Physical and
Bonding*Mental for the Full Sample with Interaction Terms set of regressions. The remaining control variables were selected using a backwards stepwise
regression procedure that chooses variables to minimize the Bayes Information Criterion. The four WLS estimators (Fixed Effects-Weight1, Fixed EffectsWeight2, Random Effects-Weight1, and Random Effects-Weight2) are described in Section 2. All four calculate cluster robust standard errors. *, **, and ***
indicate statistical significance at the 10-, 5-, and 1-percent level, respectively. The hypothesis test in the Full Sample set of regressions reports the results
of testing whether there is any difference in the estimated effects on health for the Bonding, Bridging, and Linking social capital variables.
Table 11
Mean PCC Values and the Distribution of t-values for Finer Categories of Social Capital and Health.
Obs
Mean PCC
Distribution of t-values
(1)
(2)
t < -2.00
(3)
−2.00 ≤ t ≤ 2.00
(4)
t > 2.00
(5)
Sub-categories of Cognitive Social Capital
Social trust
1,553
863
Perceived social support
Perceived social cohesion
237
179
Perceived reciprocity
173
Sense of belonging
114
Loneliness
0.045
0.038
0.043
0.053
0.038
−0.010
3.0
5.0
3.8
0.6
4.6
28.9
40.9
62.1
61.2
60.9
53.8
48.2
56.1
32.9
35.0
38.5
41.6
22.8
Sub-categories of Structural Social Capital
1,965
Social participation
1,931
Social networks
785
Social support
769
Social engagement
567
Volunteering
537
Group membership
456
Social integration
411
Social relationship
0.034
0.015
0.020
0.019
0.024
0.022
0.007
0.020
5.3
9.7
8.1
5.7
4.4
9.7
15.6
17.0
51.3
68.8
69.8
57.5
51.8
61.4
57.9
49.1
43.3
21.5
22.0
36.8
43.7
28.9
26.5
33.8
Sub-categories of Health
Mortality
Disease/Illness
Depression
0.020
0.014
0.028
9.5
5.5
8.6
62.3
75.9
59.8
28.2
18.5
31.6
2,441
830
826
nitive social capital is particularly salient for mental health,
as opposed to physical or general health.
Table 10 performs a similar set of exercises for the Bonding/Bridging/Linking social capital variables. The top panel
pools all the estimates that are based on these social capital variables. We include dummy variables for Bonding
and Bridging social capital variables. With two exceptions
(Bonding/Random Effects-Weight2; Bridging/Random EffectsWeight2), the associated coefficients are all insignificant at
the 5-percent level. Further, when we test the joint hypothesis that both coefficients equal zero, we fail to reject it
in three of the four regressions. These results suggest that
X. Xue, W.R. Reed and A. Menclova / Journal of Health Economics 72 (2020) 102317
there is little difference between bonding, bridging, and
linking social capital variables with respect to their effect
on health.
The next three panels break the full sample into three
subsamples based on health outcomes. Due to the small
number of estimates that use linking social capital variables in each of the subsamples, we are forced to combine
linking and bridging social capital as a single omitted category, so that the only included social capital variable is
Bonding. Across the three subsamples, Bonding is significant
only once, in column (4) for the physical health subsample.
When the three subsamples are combined again in the bottom panel, with interaction terms for Bonding and Physical
Health, and Bonding and Mental Health, the respective interaction terms are everywhere small in size and statistically
insignificant. This supports the finding from the top panel
in Table 10 that there is no systematic difference in how
bonding, bridging and linking social capital affect health.
Table 11 provides one last deep dive into the finer categories of the social capital and health variables. Reported
are the number of estimates associated with the respective
subcategory (Column 2), the mean PCC value for those estimates (Column 3), and the percent of associated t-statistics
that are (i) less than −2.00, (ii) between −2 and 2, and
(iii) greater than 2.00 (Columns 3–5). Not a single subcategory of social capital or health rises to the level of “small”
using Doucouliagos’ PCC (2011) guidelines. The social capital sub-categories with the largest mean PCC values are
Social trust (0.045) and Perceived reciprocity (0.053). With
only two exceptions (Social trust and Loneliness), over half
of all estimated coefficients are statistically insignificant
(−2.00 ≤ t ≤ 2.00). This is all the more noteworthy given
the large sizes of the samples underlying these estimates
(cf. Table 3).
7. Conclusion
The relationship between social capital and health has
received extensive attention in fields such as public health,
medicine, epidemiology, gerontology and other healthrelated disciplines. Hundreds of studies have reported that
social capital is significantly associated with a variety of
positive health outcomes. In contrast, the economics literature on this subject is relatively small.
The primary aim of this study has been to determine
whether the effect of social capital on health is “large” or
“small”. If “large”, then the dearth of economic research
in this area represents a gap that should be addressed. If
“small”, then this justifies the relative lack of attention this
subject has received in the health economics discipline.
We use meta-analysis to analyze the effects of 12,778
estimates from 470 studies. To place estimated effects on a
common scale, we transform the individual estimates into
partial correlation coefficients and compare these against
the “size” guidelines established by Doucouliagos (2011).
Our analysis finds that social capital is significantly related
to a variety of positive health outcomes. However, the
effects are consistently very small. This result is robust
across a wide variety of different types of social capital
(e.g., cognitive, structural, bonding, bridging, linking), and
15
for many different measures of health outcomes (e.g., mortality, disease/illnesses, depression).
One of the most compelling findings from our analysis
is that the majority of estimates in the social capital/health literature are statistically insignificant. This is
striking given the typically large sample sizes of the studies
included in our analysis: Median sample size for the studies
in our sample was 3,451. Over 75% of the estimates came
from regressions with more than 1000 observations. Large
sample sizes with high rates of statistical insignificance are
indicative of small effect sizes.
How could such a large literature grow from such small
effects? We suspect several factors are responsible. First,
estimated coefficients are difficult to interpret. Social capital variables are typically measured using Likert scale type
questions. As a result, small effect sizes are not recognized as such. Second, and perhaps as a consequence of
the first factor, researchers place undue emphasis on statistical significance. This is compounded by the fact that
studies typically include multiple social capital variables
in the same regression. The mean number of social capital variables in a regression in our sample was 6.5. The
combination of large sample sizes with multiple social
capital variables makes it relatively easy to find at least
one social capital variable that is statistically significant.
Studies rarely apply Bonferroni-type corrections when
assessing the statistical significance of multiple estimates.
This explains how most studies can conclude that social
capital has a significant effect on health, even while the
majority of estimates are insignificant.
Our study is the most extensive analysis of the extant
empirical literature on social capital and health ever undertaken. As such, it can be viewed as establishing a Bayesian
prior belief that the effect of social capital on health is very
small. The results suggest that economics is justified in not
following the leads of other disciplines in devoting substantial research resources to the study of social capital and
health.
Our study also calls into question initiatives to promote
health through social capital such as those by the WHO, the
OECD, and US Healthy People 2020 (Rocco and Suhrcke,
2012; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2010;
Keeley, 2007). In a recent systematic review, VillalongaOlives et al. (2018) note the “enthusiasm of policy makers
to implement social capital interventions to manipulate
health outcomes for the better” (p. 217). Our results suggest
that this enthusiasm may not translate into meaningful
improvements in public health.
CRediT authorship contribution statement
Xindong Xue: Conceptualization, Investigation, Project
administration, Writing - review & editing. W. Robert
Reed: Methodology, Formal analysis, Supervision, Data
curation, Validation, Writing - original draft. Andrea Menclova: Investigation, Writing - review & editing.
Acknowledgements
Xindong Xue acknowledges financial support from
the National Social Science Fund of China (Grant No.:
16
X. Xue, W.R. Reed and A. Menclova / Journal of Health Economics 72 (2020) 102317
14BRK013), the Humanities and Social Sciences Fund from
Chinese Ministry of Education (Grant No.: 20YJC630011)
and the Youth Innovation Research Project from Hubei
Province (Grant No.: T201932) W. Robert Reed acknowledges financial support from the Czech Science Foundation,
Grant 18-02513S. We are grateful to Xiaoping Ruan, Lina
Wang, Weilun Wu, Yi Xia, Junyong Yuan, and Kunpeng
Zhao for their excellent research assistance. We thank Tom
Coupé, Lorenzo Rocco, and participants at the 2019 MAERNet Colloquium for insightful comments and suggestions.
All remaining errors are our own.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
Supplementary material related to this article can be
found, in the online version, at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jhealeco.2020.102317.
References
Alesina, A., Giuliano, P., 2015. Culture and institutions. J. Econ. Lit. 53 (4),
898–944.
Algan, Y., Cahuc, P., 2010. Inherited trust and growth. Am. Econ. Rev. 100
(5), 2060–2092.
Alinaghi, N., Reed, W.R., 2018. Meta-analysis and publication bias: How
well does the FAT-PET-PEESE procedure work? Res. Synth. Methods
9 (2), 285–311.
Andini, C., Andini, M., 2018. Social capital and growth: causal evidence
from Italian municipalities. J. Econ. Geogr. 19 (3), 619–653.
Andrews, I., Kasy, M., 2017. Identification of and Correction for
Publication Bias. NBER Working Paper 23298. National Bureau of
Economic Research.
Berggren, N., Jordahl, H., 2006. Free to trust: economic freedom and
social capital. Kyklos 59 (2), 141–169.
Beugelsdijk, S., de Groot, H.L.F., van Schaik, A.B.T.M., 2004. Trust and
economic growth: a robustness analysis. Oxf. Econ. Pap. 56, 118–134.
Blakely, T., Atkinson, J., Ivory, V., Collings, S., Wilton, J.,
Howden-Chapman, P., 2006. No association of neighbourhood
volunteerism with mortality in New Zealand: a national multilevel
cohort study. Int. J. Epidemiol. 35 (4), 981–989.
Bowles, S., Gintis, H., 2002. Social capital and community governance.
Econ. J. 112 (483), F419–F436.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2010. Healthy People 2020,
https://www.healthypeople.gov/2020/topics-objectives/topic/
social-determinants-of-health. accessed at 2018/11/28.
Christensen, G., Miguel, E., 2018. Transparency, reproducibility, and the
credibility of economics research. J. Econ. Lit. 56 (3), 920–980.
Cohen, J., 1988. Statistical Power Analysis in the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd
ed. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
Coleman, J.S., 1990. Foundations of Social Theory. Harvard University
Press, Cambridge, MA.
Costa-Font, J., Mladovsky, P., 2008. Social capital and the social formation
of health-related preferences and behaviours. Health Econ. Policy
Law 3 (4), 413–427.
De Silva, M.J., McKenzie, K., Harpham, T., Huttly, S.R., 2005. Social capital
and mental illness: a systematic review. J. Epidemiol. Community
Health 59 (8), 619–627.
de Vaan, M., Frenken, K., Boschma, R., 2019d. The downside of social
capital in new industry creation. Econ. Geogr., 1–26.
Doucouliagos, H., 2011. How Large Is Large? Preliminary and Relative
Guidelines for Interpreting Partial Correlations in Economics.
Economics Series 2011/5. Deakin University, Faculty of Business and
Law, School of Accounting, Economics and Finance.
Doucouliagos, H., Paldam, M., 2013. The robust result in meta-analysis of
aid effectiveness: a response to Mekasha and Tarp. J. Dev. Stud. 49
(4), 584–587.
Doucouliagos, H., Stanley, T.D., 2013. Are all economic facts greatly
exaggerated? Theory competition and selectivity. J. Econ. Surv. 27
(2), 316–339.
Durlauf, S.N., 2002. On the empirics of social capital. Econ. J. 112 (483),
F459–F479.
Folland, S., 2008. An economic model of social capital and health. Health
Econ. Policy Law 3 (4), 333–348.
Folland, S., Kaarbøe, O., Islam, K., 2014. Chapter 3: how Do We invest in
social Capital? An exploration of an economic model of social Capital
and health. In: Folland, S., Rocco, L. (Eds.), The Economics of Social
Capital and Health. World Scientific Publishing.
Franco, A., Malhotra, N., Simonovits, G., 2014. Publication bias in the
social sciences: unlocking the file drawer. Science 345 (6203),
1502–1505.
Gilbert, K.L., Quinn, S.C., Goodman, R.M., Butler, J., Wallace, J., 2013. A
meta-analysis of social capital and health: a case for needed
research. J. Health Psychol. 18 (11), 1385–1399.
Glaeser, E.L., Laibson, D.I., Scheinkman, J.A., Soutter, C.L., 2000.
Measuring trust. Q. J. Econ. 115 (3), 811–846.
Glaeser, E.L., Laibson, D., Sacerdote, B., 2002. An economic approach to
social capital. Econ. J. 112 (483), F437–F458.
Grossman, M., 1972. On the concept of health capital and the demand for
health. J. Polit. Econ. 80 (2), 223–255.
Guiso, L., Sapienza, P., Zingales, L., 2004. The role of social capital in
financial development. Am. Econ. Rev. 94 (3), 526–556.
Gunby, P., Jin, Y., Reed, W.R., 2017. Did FDI really cause Chinese
economic growth? A meta-analysis. World Dev. 90, 242–255.
Helliwell, J.F., 2006. Well-being, social capital and public policy: what’s
new? Econ. J. 116 (510), C34–C45.
Hoi, C.K.S., Wu, Q., Zhang, H., 2019. Does social capital mitigate agency
problems? Evidence from Chief Executive Officer (CEO)
compensation. J. Financ. Econ. 133 (2), 498–519.
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T.B., Layton, J.B., 2010. Social relationships and
mortality risk: a meta-analytic review. PLoS Med. 7 (7), e1000316.
Karlan, D.S., 2005. Using experimental economics to measure social
capital and predict financial decisions. Am. Econ. Rev. 95 (5),
1688–1699.
Keeley, B., 2007. Human capital: how what you know shapes your life.
In: OECD Insights. OECD publishing, Paris.
Knack, S., Keefer, P., 1997. Does social capital have an economic payoff?
A cross-country investigation. Q. J. Econ. 112 (4), 1251–1288.
LaPorta, R., Lopez-de-Silanes, F., Shleifer, A., Vishny, R., 1997. Trust in
large organisations. American Economic Review Papers and
Proceedings 87, 333–338.
Laporte, A., 2014. Chapter 4: social capital: an economic perspective. In:
Folland, S., Rocco, L. (Eds.), The Economics of Social Capital and
Health. World Scientific Publishing.
Lindsey, C., Sheather, S., 2010. Variable selection in linear regression.
Stata J. 10 (4), 650–669.
McCoy, S.J., McDonough, I.K., Roychowdhury, P., 2019. The impact of
terrorism on social capital: evidence from the 2015 Charlie Hebdo
Paris shooting. Oxf. Bull. Econ. Stat., forthcoming.
Nannicini, T., Stella, A., Tabellini, G., Troiano, U., 2013. Social capital and
political accountability. Am. Econ. J. Econ. Policy 5 (2), 222–250.
Nyqvist, F., Pape, B., Pellfolk, T., Forsman, A.K., Wahlbeck, K., 2014.
Structural and cognitive aspects of social capital and all-cause
mortality: a meta-analysis of cohort studies. Soc. Indic. Res. 116 (2),
545–566.
Paldam, M., Svendsen, G.T., 2000. An essay on social capital: looking for
the fire behind the smoke. Eur. J. Polit. Econ. 16 (2), 339–366.
Putnam, R.D., 1993. Making Democracy Work: Civic Traditions in
Modern Italy. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.
Putnam, R.D., 1995. Bowling alone: america’s declining social capital. J.
Democr. 6, 65–78.
Reed, W.R., 2015. A Monte Carlo analysis of alternative meta-analysis
estimators in the presence of publication bias. Econ. Open-access
Open-assessment E-journal 9 (2015–30), 1–40,
http://dx.doi.org/10.5018/economics-ejournal.ja.2015-30.
Rocco, L., Suhrcke, M., 2012. Is Social Capital Good for Health? A
European Perspective. WHO Regional Office for Europe, Copenhagen.
Stanley, T.D., Doucouliagos, H., Giles, M., Heckemeyer, J.H., Johnston, R.J.,
Laroche, P., Nelson, J.P., Paldam, M., Poot, J., Pugh, G., Rosenberger,
R.S., 2013. Meta-analysis of economics research reporting guidelines.
J. Econ. Surv. 27 (2), 390–394.
Stanley, T.D., Doucouliagos, H., Ioannidis, J.P., 2017. Finding the power to
reduce publication bias. Stat. Med. 36 (10), 1580–1598.
Tabellini, G., 2010. Culture and institutions: economic development in
the regions of Europe. J. Eur. Econ. Assoc. 8 (4), 677–716.
Villalonga-Olives, E., Wind, T.R., Kawachi, I., 2018. Social capital
interventions in public health: a systematic review. Soc. Sci. Med.
212, 203–218.
Wang, S., 2019. Social capital and rotating labor associations in rural
China. China Econ. Rev. 53, 243–253.
Zak, P.J., Knack, S., 2001. Trust and growth. Econ. J. 111 (470), 295–321.