This art icle was downloaded by: [ Daina Crafa]
On: 15 May 2015, At : 09: 02
Publisher: Taylor & Francis
I nform a Lt d Regist ered in England and Wales Regist ered Num ber: 1072954 Regist ered office: Mort im er House,
37- 41 Mort im er St reet , London W1T 3JH, UK
International Review of Psychiatry
Publicat ion det ails, including inst ruct ions f or aut hors and subscript ion inf ormat ion:
ht t p: / / www. t andf online. com/ loi/ iirp20
Maternal migration and autism risk: Systematic analysis
a
Daina Craf a & Nasir Warf a
b
a
Int egrat ed Program in Neuroscience, Douglas Ment al Healt h Universit y Hospit al, McGill
Universit y, Mont real, Quebec, Canada
b
Wolf son Inst it ut e of Prevent at ive Medicine, Queen Mary Universit y of London, Bart s and
The London School of Medicine and Dent ist ry, London, UK
Published online: 15 May 2015.
To cite this article: Daina Craf a & Nasir Warf a (2015) Mat ernal migrat ion and aut ism risk: Syst emat ic analysis, Int ernat ional
Review of Psychiat ry, 27: 1, 64-71
To link to this article: ht t p: / / dx. doi. org/ 10. 3109/ 09540261. 2014. 995601
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTI CLE
Taylor & Francis m akes every effort t o ensure t he accuracy of all t he inform at ion ( t he “ Cont ent ” ) cont ained
in t he publicat ions on our plat form . However, Taylor & Francis, our agent s, and our licensors m ake no
represent at ions or warrant ies what soever as t o t he accuracy, com plet eness, or suit abilit y for any purpose of t he
Cont ent . Any opinions and views expressed in t his publicat ion are t he opinions and views of t he aut hors, and
are not t he views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of t he Cont ent should not be relied upon and
should be independent ly verified wit h prim ary sources of inform at ion. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, act ions, claim s, proceedings, dem ands, cost s, expenses, dam ages, and ot her liabilit ies what soever
or howsoever caused arising direct ly or indirect ly in connect ion wit h, in relat ion t o or arising out of t he use of
t he Cont ent .
This art icle m ay be used for research, t eaching, and privat e st udy purposes. Any subst ant ial or syst em at ic
reproduct ion, redist ribut ion, reselling, loan, sub- licensing, syst em at ic supply, or dist ribut ion in any
form t o anyone is expressly forbidden. Term s & Condit ions of access and use can be found at ht t p: / /
www.t andfonline.com / page/ t erm s- and- condit ions
International Review of Psychiatry, February 2015; 27(1): 64–71
Maternal migration and autism risk: Systematic analysis
DAINA CRAFA1 & NASIR WARFA2
1Integrated
Downloaded by [Daina Crafa] at 09:02 15 May 2015
Program in Neuroscience, Douglas Mental Health University Hospital, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec,
Canada, and 2Wolfson Institute of Preventative Medicine, Queen Mary University of London, Barts and The London
School of Medicine and Dentistry, London, UK
Abstract
Autism (AUT) is one of the most prevalent developmental disorders emerging during childhood, and can be amongst the
most incapacitating mental disorders. Some individuals with AUT require a lifetime of supervised care. Autism Speaks
reported estimated costs for 2012 at £34 billion in the UK; and $3.2 million-$126 billion in the US, Australia and
Canada. Ethnicity and migration experiences appear to increase risks of AUT and relate to underlying biological risk
factors. Sociobiological stress factors can affect the uterine environment, or relate to stress-induced epigenetic changes
during pregnancy and delivery. Epigenetic risk factors associated with AUT also include poor pregnancy conditions, low
birth weight, and congenital malformation. Recent studies report that children from migrant communities are at higher
risk of AUT than children born to non-migrant mothers, with the exception of Hispanic children. This paper provides
the first systematic review into prevalence and predictors of AUT with a particular focus on maternal migration stressors
and epigenetic risk factors. AUT rates appear higher in certain migrant communities, potentially relating to epigenetic
changes after stressful experiences. Although AUT remains a rare disorder, failures to recognize its public health urgency
and local community needs continue to leave certain cultural groups at a disadvantage.
Background
AUT is a spectrum disorder, and people with AUT
may exhibit different combinations of symptoms
with different degrees of severity. As such, it is heterogeneous in nature, and diagnosis can initially be
difficult (Constantino, 2011). To diagnose AUT,
patients are assessed for abnormal behaviours in the
following three areas: social skills, language and communication, and stereotypic behaviours.
Social deficits are well-known symptoms of AUT.
They include avoidance of social interaction, failure
to make eye contact, and inability to empathize with
another person (Chevallier et al., 2012; Green et al.,
2000). Even when AUT is mild to moderate, the
disorder can cause significant distress for patients
and their families. It can prevent bonding, act as an
impediment to learning, and substantially delay the
child’s development (Dunst et al., 2011; Schipul
et al., 2011). Symptoms of AUT emerge before the
child is two years of age, and repeated failure to make
eye contact or to respond when called by name are
among the earliest indicators (American Psychiatric
Association, 2013).
These deficits are biopsychosocial in nature and
can prevent the patient from forming, or sustaining,
meaningful relationships, which can cause problems
when seeking jobs or medical care in adulthood
(Grant et al., 2001; Green et al., 2000). Other
symptoms of AUT include language and communication deficits. Severe language disability frequently
co-occurs with the disorder, affecting approximately
38% of all children with AUT (Centers for Disease
Control and Prevention, 2014). In some cases, children with AUT may be unable to speak or even communicate non-verbally (American Psychiatric
Association, 2013). Moreover, unless treated very
early in life, patients are unlikely to learn to speak
or effectively communicate (Virués-Ortega, 2010).
Stereotypy, a third symptom of AUT, describes
repetitive behaviours frequently exhibited by patients,
such as mirror gazing or rocking, that provide solitary self-stimulation and may be repeated for long
periods of time (Goldman et al., 2009; Matson et al.,
2009a).
Moreover, the severity of an individual case of
AUT is generally described as either ‘high functioning’ or ‘low functioning’, with children with intellectual disability (ID) falling into the latter category.
High functioning AUT is generally associated
with intelligence quotient (IQ) scores higher than 70
Correspondence: Daina Crafa, MSc, Integrated Program in Neuroscience, Douglas Mental Health Institute, McGill University, Frank B. Common Pavilion,
6875 Boulevard LaSalle, Room F-1145, Montreal, Quebec, Canada, H4H 1R3. Tel: ⫹ 001 514-761-6131. Fax: ⫹ 001 514-888-4064. E-mail: daina.crafa@
mail.mcgill.ca
(Received 22 August 2014 ; accepted 25 November 2014 )
ISSN 0954–0261 print/ISSN 1369–1627 online © 2015 Institute of Psychiatry
DOI: 10.3109/09540261.2014.995601
Maternal migration and autism risk
(e.g. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention,
2014; Gillberg & Billstedt, 2000; Hallett et al., forthcoming; Matson et al., 2009b; Mazurek & Kanne,
2010; Sukhodolsky et al., 2008; Weisbrot et al., 2005).
Low functioning AUT is associated with IQ scores
below 70, and impairments in speech or daily living
skills are most often observed in this group (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). The severity of
behavioural problems in the three diagnostic domains
(i.e. social, communication, and stereotypy) have
been shown to correlate with low IQ, and it has been
proposed that children with normal or high IQs are
better able to develop compensatory mechanisms to
overcome their deficits (Fein et al., 2013).
65
et al., 2009; Gillberg, & Gillberg, 1983, 1996; Keen
et al., 2010). Although a few studies show equal or
lower rates of AUT in migrant communities (Croen
et al., 2002; Dealberto, 2011; Schieve et al., 2012),
these studies have generally focused on specific
ethnic groups. A systematic review of the literature
is necessary to examine global trends and their
implications.
This comprehensive analysis is aimed to update
the current literature on the prevalence and predictors of AUT, particularly focusing on the associations between maternal migration stressors,
epigenetic changes and increased rates of AUT in
migrant communities.
Downloaded by [Daina Crafa] at 09:02 15 May 2015
Risk factors for AUT
Several environmental and biological risk factors
have been associated with AUT (Croen et al., 2002;
Haglund & Källén, 2011; Hultman et al., 2002;
Williams et al., 2008). Suboptimal birth or poor
pregnancy conditions increase risk for AUT. Low
Apgar score, low birth weight, caesarean section, premature birth, congenial malformation, and multiple
births have also been linked with the development of
AUT (ibid.). Higher parental age (both paternal and
maternal age) was reported as a key indicator of risk
(Lauritsen et al., 2005; Magnusson et al., 2012;
Williams et al., 2008). In both cases, increased age
is related to degradation of reproductive cells resulting in suboptimal pregnancy conditions and potential epigenetic changes, causing AUT (Durkin et al.,
2008). While genetic sequences remain stable across
the lifespan, epigenetic changes are alterable and
often heritable modifications that are made to DNA
processes, including methylation and histone deacetylation; such modifications may alter the expression
of a gene. Numerous studies have linked epigenetic
changes to social or environmental causes, including
migration, and demonstrated that they may be passed
down to later generations (e.g. Dealberto, 2007;
Furrow et al., 2011; McEwen et al., 2012; Tahira &
Agius, 2012; Uddin et al., 2010). Epigenetic mutations have recently been identified as causing poor
birth conditions linked with AUT (Schanen, 2006;
Walker et al., 2013), and it is hypothesized that
increased risk of autism may be related to epigenetic
changes in some communities.
Some studies suggest that the stress from maternal migration could potentially initiate such epigenetic changes – sometimes termed the ‘migration
theory of autism’ (Gardener et al., 2009; Keen et al.,
2010; Kinney et al., 2008; Magnusson et al., 2012).
Many studies have reported that children in migrant
communities are at higher risk for autism, and it has
been theorized that maternal migration prior to giving birth may further increase this risk (Gardener
Methods
We carried out a systematic review into the world
literature examining migration as a risk factor for
AUT. We used several databases including PubMed,
PyscINFO, Google Scholar and Embase to search
for articles that met our inclusion criteria. Papers
were eligible for inclusion if they had peer-reviewed
population data on AUT and maternal migration. We
excluded papers if maternal migration status was not
clearly defined (e.g. studies using vague terms such
as: ‘one parent’ migrated). Studies of mothers with
both pre- and post-pregnancy migration statuses
were included in the review.
We used comprehensive search terms to identify
relevant papers. These included migration, autism,
autism migrant, migration and autism, and maternal
migration and autism (see Appendix 1 for more
search terms). All papers published in English which
met the initial inclusion criteria were included in the
final analysis (see Table 1). We selected articles if they
had a clearly defined method of data collection, analysis and reporting. Studies without clearly tested
validity and reliability constructs were excluded from
the review.
AUT is defined in accordance with the DSM-IV
and ICD-10 diagnostic criteria for ‘Autistic disorder’,
which is traditionally defined as Kanner-type autism
and less subject to heterogeneity across diagnostic
manuals. Studies of Asperger syndrome, pervasive
developmental disorder – not otherwise specified
(PDD-NOS), or other disorders on the AUT spectrum (e.g. Rett syndrome) were excluded. Articles
were only considered for inclusion if they reported
data for these disorders separately. Since some data
collection occurred during earlier versions of the
DSM manual, definitions of AUT from the DSM-III
(American Psychiatric Association, 1980) and ICD-8
(World Health Organization, 1968), as well as later
editions (e.g. DSM-5, American Psychiatric Association, 2013), were considered valid for this study.
66
D. Crafa & N. Warfa
Downloaded by [Daina Crafa] at 09:02 15 May 2015
Table 1. Studies that met inclusion criteria and were selected for the final review.
Study
Country of
migration
Diagnostic manual
Williams et al., 2007
Australia
DSM-IV
Population registry linked with
medical records
Lauritsen et al.,
2005
Denmark
ICD-8, ICD-10
Danish civil registration system
Van der Ven et al.,
2012
Netherlands
DSM-IV
Population registry linked with
psychiatry case register
Haglund, 2011
Sweden
DSM-III, DSM-IV,
ICD-10
Hultman, 2002
Sweden
ICD-9
Magnusson et al.,
2013
Sweden
DSM-IV
Swedish medical birth registry
linked with child and youth
centre registry
Swedish medical birth registry
linked with the Swedish
inpatient registry
Stockholm youth cohort linked
with official registries
Hassan et al., 2012
UK
ICD-10
Keen et al., 2010
UK
ICD-10
Croen et al., 2002
USA
DSM-III-R
ICD-9-CM
Schieve et al., 2012
USA
Parent-reported
DSM diagnosis
Data source
Sample
All children born in New South
Wales during the years of data
collection
All children born in Denmark
under 10 years of age during the
years of data collection
All children born in the urban and
sub-rural areas surrounding
Utrecht during the years of data
collection
All children born in Malmö during
the years of data collection
All children born in Sweden during
the years of data collection
All children living in Stockholm
County ages 0–17 years during
the years of data collection
All children born in the UK who
were also registered in the source
databases
Autism Service databases were
compared with local public
health demographic data and
population reports projections
Wandsworth and Lambeth
paediatric child development
service records
California Department of
Developmental Services linked
with California live birth
certificate files
National survey of legal guardians
We analysed AUT rates from all of the samples
reported by the papers reviewed (Table 2). For easy
comparison, rates higher than 1% of the total sample
(e.g. higher than the current global average) were
classified as high, while rates lower than 1% were
All children born in the UK during
the years of data collection whose
records were on file in data
sources
All children born in California
during the years of data
collection
All children born in the USA aged
3-17 who were randomly selected
to participate in the 2007
National Survey of Children’s
Health
classified as low. Relative rates of children with AUT
born to migrant and non-migrant mothers were
compared in the following ways. First, we calculated
a sum total of combined rates across all articles, and
then the total values of maternal migration status
Table 2. Rates of AUT based on the reported findings from each study. Each rate represents the AUT
N-value relative to the total study population (AUT⫹ controls).
Country
First author
AUT population
N-value
All studies
Australia
Denmark
Netherlands
Sweden
Sweden
Sweden
UK
UK
USA
USA
–
Williams
Lauritsen
Van der Ven
Haglund
Hultman
Magnusson
Hassan
Keen
Croen
Schieve
11,185
182
796
137
157
403
3,918
356
388
4,356
493
Control population
N-value
4,051,581
85,503
76,399
160,009
68,807
2,020
40,034
61,417
23,825
3,493,514
40,052
AUT (%)
0.28
0.21
1.03
0.09
0.23
16.63
8.91
0.58
1.60
0.12
1.21
AUT
(high/low)
low
low
average
low
low
high
high
low
high
low
high
Maternal migration and autism risk
(children of migrant mothers versus children of nonmigrant mothers). For each study we also calculated
the rate of AUT per 100,000 so that no single study
inflated results from across maternal migration status
and ethnic groups.
Results
Downloaded by [Daina Crafa] at 09:02 15 May 2015
Sample and demography
Ten large-scale population studies with high
N-values conducted in six countries met our inclusion criteria. These studies had high statistical power:
N ⫽ 4,062,766 (AUT⫹ control), N ⫽ 11,185 (AUT).
Nearly all of these studies used data from birth registries and medical records (Croen et al., 2002;
Haglund & Källén, 2011; Hassan, 2012; Hultman
et al., 2002; Keen et al., 2010; Lauritsen et al., 2005;
Magnusson et al., 2012; Schieve et al., 2012; Van der
Ven et al., 2013; Williams et al., 2008). With the
exception of one study conducted in Australia, all
populations lived in Western Europe (N ⫽ 8) and the
USA (N ⫽ 2).
The total rate of AUT, which represents the
summed total of all studies included, was 0.28%. The
rates of AUT for each sample ranged from 0.09–
16.67% (SD ⫽ 5.46), based on the populations
reported by each article. Studies with high rates of
AUT had a mean of 7.09 (N ⫽ 4, SD ⫽ 7.28); studies
reporting low rates of AUT had a mean of 0.25
(N ⫽ 5, SD ⫽ 0.19). The variations between the studies reporting high and low rates were not explained
by differences in sample size, data collection methods
and country of origin.
We compared the rate of AUT for children born
to non-migrant mothers with that of children born
to migrant mothers (Table 3). Three of the four studies reporting high rates of AUT at population level
also reported similar high rates for children born to
both migrant- and non-migrant mothers. A fourth
study found higher rates for the total AUT population; and the same higher rates for children born
to native mothers, but reported lower AUT rate for
children born to migrant mothers.
On average, studies reported approximately
equal rates of AUT for children with maternal and
non-maternal migration status (0.1% to 0.2% respectively). Half of the studies showed no significant
differences between AUT rates in children born to
migrant and non-migrant mothers (Croen et al.,
2002; Hassan, 2012; Magnusson et al., 2012; Van der
Ven et al., 2013; Williams et al., 2008). Of the remaining studies, two reported higher rates of AUT for
children born to non-migrant mothers (Lauritsen
et al., 2005; Schieve et al., 2012), and three reported
higher rates for children of migrant-born mothers
(Haglund & Källén, 2011; Hultman et al., 2002;
Keen et al., 2010).
We calculated the reported N-values from each
study and adjusted the rate of AUT per 100,000
children and therefore controlled for the effects of
different sample sizes and ethnic backgrounds. The
results showed higher rates of AUT if children had
migrant mothers (2.69%) compared with children of
non-migrant mothers (0.91%). Six out of ten studies
found that giving birth post-maternal migration significantly raised risks of AUT (Haglund & Källén,
2011; Hultman et al., 2002; Keen et al., 2010;
Lauritsen et al., 2005; Schieve et al., 2012; Williams
et al., 2008), with the exception of children born to
Hispanic migrant mothers who had lower rates of
AUT (compared to all other migrant groups).
Discussion
This study reviewed the prevalence and predictors of
AUT in children born to migrant and non-migrant
mothers in the international literature to clarify
global trends. The results indicated maternal migration status as a strong predictor of AUT for children
Table 3. Rates of AUT maternal migration status. Rates represent AUT N-value relative to the total
AUT population (AUT⫹ controls).
Country of
migration
All studies
Australia
Denmark
Netherlands
Sweden
Sweden
Sweden
UK
UK
USA
USA
67
First author
AUT (%) for children
of non-migrant-born
mothers
AUT
(high/low)
AUT (%) for children
of migrant-born
mothers
AUT
(high/low)
Williams
Lauritsen
Van der Ven
Haglund
Hultman
Magnusson
Hassan
Keen
Croen
Schieve
0.34
0.19
1.00
0.12
0.31
15.66
9.03
0.63
1.14
0.14
1.25
low
low
average
low
low
high
high
low
high
low
high
0.19
0.28
1.23
0.05
0.40
28.57
8.48
0.56
2.73
0.10
0.85
low
low
high
low
low
high
high
low
high
low
low
Downloaded by [Daina Crafa] at 09:02 15 May 2015
68
D. Crafa & N. Warfa
of migrant communities (see Table 4). At times,
migration status comes across as a proxy for ethnicity. Ethnicity was a strong confounder. However,
when we controlled for ethnic status, we observed
higher rates of AUT among children with migrant
mothers (2.69% versus 0.91%), with the exception
of children born to Hispanic migrant mothers who
consistently showed lower AUT rates against all
other ethnic groups; perhaps showing the differential
effects of maternal migration on AUT. In other
words, ethnicity and biological risk factors alone cannot account for the differential rates of AUT found
in children born to migrant mothers.
Several previous studies link maternal migration
stressors with obstetric complications during pregnancy and labour (Binder et al., 2012a, 2012b, 2013;
Essen et al., 2011). These include stressors linked
with sociocultural conditions, beliefs and practices
(Magnusson et al., 2012; Van der Ven et al., 2013),
for example psychological and medical distress that
can accompany common experiences, such as targeted discrimination, or culturally specific experiences, such as forced female circumcision. Mismatch
expectations between migrant groups and healthcare
providers (Warfa et al., 2006, 2012), cultural or
community misunderstandings of care procedures,
inaccessible healthcare services, language and communication barriers (Bhui et al. , 2003, 2007; Warfa
et al., 2006), and delayed help-seeking behaviour
(Bhui et al., 2003; Binder et al, 2012a, 2012b., 2013;
Essen et al., 2011; Warfa et al., 2006, 2012) may all
lead to stressors that induce epigenetic changes during
pregnancy and delivery; with associated risk of AUT.
Sociocultural factors, such as discrimination, have
been used to explain the higher maternal morbidity
and mortality rates found in migrant communities.
For example, a major enquiry into maternal and
child health in Sweden reported maternal mortality
rate that was six times higher than the general population among migrant groups such as Somali migrants
(Binder et al., 2012a, 2012b, 2013; Essen et al.,
2011; Warfa et al., 2012). The same mechanisms that
link the above socio-economic risk factors with a
higher rate of maternal mortality and morbidity in
migrant communities can also explain the differential
AUT rates in children of migrant mothers. In other
words, sociocultural related stressors such as discrimination that occur before or during pregnancy
and labour can be hypothesized to explain the higher
prevalence rate of AUT in immigrant populations.
In contrast, nurturing sociocultural conditions
may potentially have protective effects on AUT rates.
A number of studies report that Hispanic women are
significantly less likely to have a baby with low birth
weight compared with other ethnic groups (AcevedoGarcia et al., 2005; Collins & Shay, 1994; FuentesAfflick & Lurie, 1997). Sometimes called the Latina
or Hispanic paradox, this trend has been observed
even when the mother has migrated from stressful
conditions such as from a developing country or one
with a low human development index. The Latina
paradox is frequently observed in Hispanic women
moving from Mexico to the USA, and multiple
researchers attribute it to the relief of moving from
a stressful sociopolitical situation to the close-knit
Hispanic communities typical in the USA. This phenomenon appears to occur despite the high rates of
unemployment and discrimination sometimes faced
by these communities, which is generally still less
than what they faced in their home country. Low
birth-weight neonates may be up to seven times more
likely to develop AUT (Movsas et al., 2013). Positive
relationships between the large social network sizes
characteristic of Hispanic communities and higher
birth weights have been observed, as well as inverse
relationships between social network size and AUT
Table 4. Risk factors for AUT.
N studies
report risk
Maternal migration
7
Ethnic background
Suboptimal pregnancy or birth conditions
Parental age
Caesarean section
Low APGAR score
Low birth weight
Migrating from developing country
Premature birth
Immediate family member with AUT or related disorder
Congenital malformation
Maternal education
Multiple births
Smoking during pregnancy
Urbanization
4
4
3
2
2
2
2
2
1
1
1
1
1
1
First author
Williams, Lauritsen, Haglund, Hultman, Magnusson,
Keen, Schieve
Hassan, Keen, Croen, Schieve
Haglund, Hultman, Williams, Croen
Williams, Lauritsen, Magnusson
Haglund, Hultman
Haglund, Hultman
Haglund, Hultman
Van der Ven, Magnusson
Williams, Haglund
Lauritsen
Hultman
Croen
Croen
Hultman
Lauritsen
Downloaded by [Daina Crafa] at 09:02 15 May 2015
Maternal migration and autism risk
rates (Dyer et al., 2011; Palmer et al., 2010). Accordingly, it seems plausible that nurturing sociocultural
conditions may protect the reproductive environment and reduce the chances of having a child with
AUT after maternal migration. Some studies show a
positive relationship between social network size and
higher birth weights (Dyer et al., 2011) and an
inverse relationship with AUT rates (Palmer et al.,
2010). Taken together, social environment may have
protective effects against epigenetic changes related
to birthing conditions and AUT. Moreover, these
findings suggest that the stress response may emerge
as a dynamic between the migrating individual and
their new community, rather than being restricted to
the act of migration.
In conclusion, the link between AUT and maternal
migration status is inconsistent with AUT’s epigenetic origins and emergence during early childhood.
We further propose that maternity migration stressors and poor environments may cause epigenetic
changes during pregnancy and delivery that lead to
AUT. The caveats are crucial modifications to the
epigenetic literature of AUT. The revised hypothesis
includes the importance of recognizing adverse
effects of sociocultural practices on mental and physical health and the potential benefits of nurturing
social communities.
Premenopausal migrant women and their families may particularly benefit from public health outreach and AUT awareness campaigns. Such health
education programmes can highlight AUT risk
after migration and other stressful sociocultural
experiences, as well as the benefits of a nurturing
community. This can range from displaying multilingual flyers at migration offices to training doctors to talk with migrant mothers about AUT risk
factors, benefits of early diagnosis and treatment
methods. These efforts can be geared particularly
to non-Hispanic migrants, who appear to be at
higher risk, as well migrants who are culturally disinclined to seek AUT treatment. They may also be
useful for Caucasian migrants, who may not be
aware that they may also be at higher risk for having a child with AUT. Early intervention techniques
have been developed to teach young children compensatory mechanisms for coping with the disorder
(e.g. Dunlap, 1999; Schreibman, 2000). These
techniques are the only proven treatment for AUT.
They can significantly improve quality of life and,
in some cases, can virtually eliminate AUT symptoms when administered early enough in childhood. As such, early diagnosis of the disorder is
crucial to children’s improvement and future wellbeing (Ventola et al., 2006). Modest steps such as
these have the potential to greatly improve AUT
management in general, and particularly among
migrant communities.
69
Limitations
There are a few limitations to this systematic review.
First, the body of literature on migration and AUT
is scant. Secondly, the migration theory of autism
has gained relative presence in the global mental
health literature, and yet this theory is based largely
on research coming from studies with methodological limitations and convenient sample sizes. Likewise, the findings of the larger studies reported in
this review are not necessarily representative of all
migrant communities living in different countries,
nor are the host samples necessarily generalizable
to other host communities. Moreover, ethnicity
may not be the optimal method of group delineation when assessing autism risk. Evaluations of specific sociocultural communities, political climates,
or genetic traits may provide more nuanced assessments. The findings should be read with these limitations in mind. Third, diagnostic criteria for
autism have changed across diagnostic manuals
and may result in some heterogeneity in our sample, despite our best efforts to control for this.
Finally, while maternal migration stressors may
lead to adverse epigenetic changes, there also
appear to be protective mechanisms that prevent
this negative effect (i.e. the Hispanic paradox).
The Hispanic protective paradox requires further
collaborative investigation.
Collaborations between life sciences,
social sciences and medical humanities
While the race for the cure continues, the disadvantages faced by migrant communities can be
lessened through improved knowledge, wider
knowledge disseminations and effective community
engagement. AUT is a rare illness, and yet, failure
to recognize its public health urgency will perpetuate further myths and misconceptions of the development and diagnosis of AUT. With previous
reports of misguided cause–effect associations
between AUT and MMR vaccines, autism mythology already fills in the current scientific gaps. We
call for joint collaborative research by life scientists,
social scientists and medical humanists. Interdisciplinary research could consider investigating
AUT from diverse biological, social, political and
cultural perspectives; and the extent to which these
bio-sociocultural stressors are capable of triggering
adverse epigenetic changes that are linked with
AUT risks.
Declaration of interest: The authors report no
conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the paper.
70
D. Crafa & N. Warfa
Downloaded by [Daina Crafa] at 09:02 15 May 2015
References
Acevedo-Garcia, D., Soobader, M.J., & Berkman, L.F. (2005).
The differential effect of foreign-born status on low birth weight
by race/ethnicity and education. Pediatrics, 115, e20–30.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.). Washington, D.C.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). The Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.). Washington,
DC: American Psychiatric Association.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). The Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed.). Washington,
DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Bhui, K., Mohamud, S., Warfa, N., Craig, T.J., & Stansfeld, S.A.
(2003). Cultural adaptation of mental health measures: Improving the quality of clinical practice and research. British Journal
of Psychiatry, 183, 184–186.
Bhui, K., Warfa, N., Edonya, P., McKenzie, K., & Bhugra, D.
(2007). Cultural competence in mental health care: A review
of model evaluations. BMC Health Services Research, 7, 15.
Binder, P., Borné, Y., Johnsdotter, S., & Essén, B. (2012b). Shared
language is essential: Communication in a multiethnic obstetric
care setting. Journal of Health Communication, 17, 1171–1186.
Binder, P., Johnsdotter, S., & Essén, B. (2013). More than re-establishing the partner relationship: Intimate aftercare for Somali
parents in diaspora. Midwifery, 29, 863–870.
Binder, P., Johnsdotter, S., & Essén, B. (2012a). Conceptualising
the prevention of adverse obstetric outcomes among
immigrants using the ‘three delays’ framework in a high-income
context. Social Science and Medicine, 75, 2028–2036.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2014) Autism
spectrum disorder: Data and statistics. Retrieved from: http://www.
cdc.gov/ncbddd/autism/ data.html
Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E.S., & Schultz, R.T.
(2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 16, 231–239.
Collins, J.W., & Shay, D.K. (1994). Prevalence of low birth weight
among Hispanic infants with United States-born and foreignborn mothers: The effect of urban poverty. American Journal of
Epidemiology, 139, 184–192.
Constantino, J.N. (2011). The quantitative nature of autistic social
impairment. Pediatric Research, 69, 55R–62R.
Croen, L.A., Grether, J.K., & Selvin, S. (2002). Descriptive epidemiology of autism in a California population: Who is at risk?
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 217–224.
Dealberto, M.J. (2007). Why are immigrants at increased risk for
psychosis? Vitamin D insufficiency, epigenetic mechanisms, or
both? Medical Hypotheses, 68, 259–267.
Dealberto, M.J. (2011). Prevalence of autism according to maternal immigrant status and ethnic origin. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 123, 339–348.
Dunlap, G. (1999). Consensus, engagement, and family involvement for young children with autism. Journal of the Association
for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 24, 222–225.
Dunst, C.J., Trivette, C.M., & Masiello, T. (2011). Exploratory
investigation of the effects of interest-based learning on
the development of young children with autism. Autism, 15,
295–305.
Durkin, M.S., Maenner, M.J., Newschaffer, C.J., Lee, L.C., Cunniff , C.M., Daniels, J.L., … Schieve, L.A. (2008). Advanced
parental age and the risk of autism spectrum disorder. American
Journal of Epidemiology, 168, 1268–1276.
Dyer, J.M., Hunter, R., & Murphy, P.A. (2011). Relationship of
social network size to infant birth weight in Hispanic and
non-Hispanic women. Journal of Immigrant and Minority Health,
13, 487–493.
Essén, B., Binder, P., & Johnsdotter, S. (2011). An anthropological
analysis of the perspectives of Somali women in the West
and their obstetric care providers on caesarean birth. Journal of
Psychosomatic Obstetrics and Gynecology, 32, 10–18.
Fein, D., Barton, M., Eigsti, I.M., Kelley, E., Naigles, L.,
Schultz, R.T., … Tyson, K. (2013). Optimal outcome in individuals with a history of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 54, 195–205.
Fuentes-Afflick, E., & Lurie, P. (1997). Low birth weight and
Latino ethnicity: examining the epidemiologic paradox. Archives
of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 151, 665–674.
Furrow, R.E., Christiansen, F.B., & Feldman, M.W. (2011). Environment-sensitive epigenetics and the heritability of complex
diseases. Genetics, 189, 1377–1387.
Gardener, H., Spiegelman, D., & Buka, S.L. (2009). Prenatal
risk factors for autism: Comprehensive meta-analysis. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 195, 7–14.
Gillberg, C., & Billstedt, E. (2000). Autism and Asperger
syndrome: Coexistence with other clinical disorders. Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 102, 321–330.
Gillberg, I.C., & Gillberg, C. (1996). Autism in immigrants: A
population-based study from Swedish rural and urban areas.
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 40, 24–31.
Gillberg, I.C., & Gillberg, C. (1983). Infantile autism: A total
population study of reduced optimality in the pre, peri-, and
neonatal period. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
13, 153–166.
Goldman, S., Wang, C., Salgado, M.W., Greene, P.E., Kim, M.,
& Rapin, I. (2009). Motor stereotypies in children with autism
and other developmental disorders. Developmental Medicine and
Child Neurology, 51, 30–38.
Grant, C., Addington, J., Addington, D., & Konnert, C. (2001).
Social functioning in first-and multiepisode schizophrenia.
Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 46, 746–749.
Green, J., Gilchrist, A., Burton, D., & Cox, A. (2000). Social and
psychiatric functioning in adolescents with Asperger syndrome
compared with conduct disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 279–293.
Haglund, N.G., & Källén, K.B. (2011). Risk factors for autism
and Asperger syndrome: Perinatal factors and migration.
Autism, 15, 163–183.
Hallett, V., Lecavalier, L., Sukhodolsky, D.G., Cipriano, N.,
Aman, M.G., McCracken, J.T., … Scahill, L. (2013). Exploring
the manifestations of anxiety in children with autism spectrum
disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43,
2341–2352.
Hassan, M. (2012). Predicting the prevalence of Autism among
ethnic groups. Archives of Childhood Disease, 97, A95–A96.
Hultman, C.M., Sparén, P., & Cnattingius, S. (2002). Perinatal
risk factors for infantile autism. Epidemiology, 13, 417–423.
Keen, D.V., Reid, F.D., & Arnone, D. (2010). Autism, ethnicity
and maternal immigration. British Journal of Psychiatry, 196,
274–281.
Kinney, D.K., Munir, K.M., Crowley, D.J., & Miller, A.M. (2008).
Prenatal stress and risk for autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 1519–1532.
Lauritsen, M.B., Pedersen, C.B., & Mortensen, P.B. (2005).
Effects of familial risk factors and place of birth on the risk
of autism: A nationwide register based study. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 963–971.
Magnusson, C., Rai, D., Goodman, A., Lundberg, M., Idring, S.,
Svensson, A., … Dalman, C. (2012). Migration and autism
spectrum disorder: Population-based study. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 201, 109–115.
Matson, J.L., Dempsey, T., & Fodstad, J.C. (2009a). Stereotypies
and repetitive/restrictive behaviours in infants with autism
and pervasive developmental disorder. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 12, 122–127.
Matson, J.L., Dempsey, T., & Fodstad, J.C. (2009b). The effect of
autism spectrum disorders on adaptive independent living skills
Downloaded by [Daina Crafa] at 09:02 15 May 2015
Maternal migration and autism risk
in adults with severe intellectual disability. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30, 1203–1211.
Mazurek, M.O., & Kanne, S.M. (2010). Friendship and internalizing symptoms among children and adolescents with ASD.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 1512–1520.
McEwen, B.S., Eiland, L., Hunter, R.G., & Miller, M.M. (2012).
Stress and anxiety: Structural plasticity and epigenetic regulation as a consequence of stress. Neuropharmacology, 62, 3–12.
Movsas, T.Z., Pinto-Martin, J.A., Whitaker, A.H., Feldman, J.F.,
Lorenz, J.M., Korzeniewski, S.J., … Paneth, N. (2013). Autism
spectrum disorder is associated with ventricular enlargement
in a low birth weight population. Journal of Pediatrics, 163,
73–78.
Palmer, R.F., Walker, T., Mandell, D., Bayles, B., & Miller, C.S.
(2010). Explaining low rates of autism among Hispanic schoolchildren in Texas. American Journal of Public Health, 100, 270–272.
Schanen, N.C. (2006). Epigenetics of autism spectrum disorders.
Human Molecular Genetics, 15, R138-R150.
Schieve, L.A., Boulet, S.L., Blumberg, S.J., Kogan, M.D., YearginAllsopp, M., Boyle, C.A., .. Rice, C. (2012). Association between
parental nativity and autism spectrum disorder among US-born
non-Hispanic white and Hispanic children, 2007 National Survey
of Children’s Health. Disability and Health Journal, 5, 18–25.
Schipul, S.E., Williams, D.L., Keller, T.A., Minshew, N.J., &
Just, M.A. (2011). Distinctive neural processes during learning
in autism. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 937–950.
Schreibman, L. (2000). Intensive behavioral/psychoeducational
treatments for autism: Research needs and future directions.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 373–378.
Sukhodolsky, D.G., Scahill, L., Gadow, K.D., Arnold, L.E.,
Aman, M.G., McDougle, C.J., …Vitiello, B. (2008). Parentrated anxiety symptoms in children with pervasive developmental disorders: Frequency and association with core autism
symptoms and cognitive functioning. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology, 36, 117–128.
Tahira, A., & Agius, M. (2012). Epigenetics and migration-considerations based on the incidence of psychosis in South Asians
in Luton, England. Psychiatria Danubina, 24, S194–196.
Uddin, M., Aiello, A.E., Wildman, D.E., Koenen, K.C., Pawelec,
G., de los Santosa, R., … Galea, S. (2010). Epigenetic and
immune function profiles associated with posttraumatic stress
Appendix 1.
Broad search terms
Autism migration
Autism migrant
Autism migration
Autism migrant
Autism ethnic
Autism ethnicity
Autism epidemiology
Maternal migration autism
Maternal migration autism
Autism refugee
Specific search terms
Autism U.K. migrant
Autism U.S. migrant
Africa autism migration
Asia autism migration
Hispanic autism migration
Caribbean autism migration
Arab autism migration
Autism U.K. migrant
Autism U.S. migrant
Africa autism migration
Asia autism migration
Hispanic autism migration
Caribbean autism migration
Arab autism migration
71
disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107,
9470–9475.
Van der Ven, E., Termorshuizen, F., Laan, W., Breetvelt, E.J.,
Os, J., & Selten, J.P. (2013). An incidence study of diagnosed
autism spectrum disorders among immigrants to the Netherlands. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 128, 54–60.
Ventola, P.E., Kleinman, J., Pandey, J., Barton, M., Allen, S.,
Green, J., … Fein, D. (2006). Agreement among four
diagnostic instruments for autism spectrum disorders in
toddlers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36,
839–847.
Virués-Ortega, J. (2010). Applied behavior analytic intervention
for autism in early childhood: Meta-analysis, meta-regression
and dose–response meta-analysis of multiple outcomes. Clinical
Psychology Review, 30, 387–399.
Walker, C.K., Anderson, K.W., Milano, K.M., Ye, S.,
Tancredi, D.J., Pessah, I.N., … Kliman, H.J. (2013). Trophoblast inclusions are significantly increased in the placentas of
children in families at risk for autism. Biological Psychiatry, 74,
204–211.
Warfa N., Bhui K., Craig T., Mohamud S., Stansfeld S.,
McCrone T., … Thornicroft, G. (2006). Post-migration geographical mobility, mental health and health service utilization
among Somali refugees in the UK: A qualitative study. Health
and Place, 12, 503–515.
Warfa, N., Curtis, S., Watters, C., Carswell, K., Ingleby, D.,
& Bhui, K. (2012). Migration experiences, employment
status and psychological distress among Somali immigrants:
A mixed-method international study. BMC Public Health,
12, 749.
Weisbrot, D.M., Gadow, K.D., DeVincent, C.J., & Pomeroy, J.
(2005). The presentation of anxiety in children with pervasive
developmental disorders. Journal of Child and Adolescent
Psychopharmacology, 15, 477–496.
Williams, K., Helmer, M., Duncan, G.W., Peat, J.K., &
Mellis, C.M. (2008). Perinatal and maternal risk factors for
autism spectrum disorders in New South Wales, Australia.
Child: Care, Health and Development, 34, 249–256.
World Health Organization. (1968). ICD-8: International Statistical
Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (8th ed.).
New York, NY: World Health Organization.