Cognition 136 (2015) 337–349
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Cognition
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT
Metamemory appraisals in autobiographical event recall
Alan Scoboria a,⇑, Jennifer M. Talarico b, Lisa Pascal a
a
b
Department of Psychology, University of Windsor, Ontario, Canada
Department of Psychology, Lafayette College, Easton, PA, United States
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Received 30 December 2013
Revised 22 October 2014
Accepted 17 November 2014
Available online 16 December 2014
Keywords:
Event memory
Recollection
Autobiographical belief
Confidence
Accuracy
Metamemory
a b s t r a c t
Two studies examined whether belief in the occurrence of events, recollecting events, and
belief in the accuracy of recollections are distinct aspects of autobiographical remembering. In Study 1, 299 student participants received a cue to recall five childhood events, after
which they rated each event on these constructs and other characteristics associated with
remembering. Structural equation modelling revealed that variance in ratings was best
explained by the three anticipated latent variables. In Study 2, an online sample of 1026
adults recalled and rated a childhood event and an event about which they were somehow
uncertain. Confirmatory modelling replicated the three latent variables. The relationship of
key predictors (perceptual detail, spatial detail, re-experiencing, and event plausibility) to
the latent variables confirmed the distinction. These studies demonstrate that belief in
occurrence and belief in accuracy appraisals are distinct, the former indexing the truth status of the event and the latter the degree to which the event representation accurately
reflects prior experience. Further, they suggest that belief in accuracy indexes the monitoring of the quality of recollections.
Ó 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
For at least the past half-century, researchers have done
an admirable job of describing the phenomenology of
autobiographical memory (AM). Features of remembering
like vividness and visual perspective are well described.
Further work has examined how these properties are
influenced by event features (e.g., emotion or time since
occurrence) or by cognitive processes (e.g., imagination).
Yet, Baddeley (2012) recently encouraged researchers in
the area to integrate such diverse topics into broader theoretical understandings. Some theoretical and empirical
efforts to integrate these individual findings into a more
complete picture of AM processing have been made
(Conway, Singer, & Tagini, 2004; Rubin, 2006). Importantly,
⇑ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, University of
Windsor, 401 Sunset, Windsor, Ontario N9B 3P4, Canada. Tel.: +1 519 253
3000x4090.
E-mail address:
[email protected] (A. Scoboria).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.028
0010-0277/Ó 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
integration must be done with a clear understanding of the
dissociable constructs that contribute to the experience of
remembering events.
In this work, we continue this line of inquiry via discussion of three concepts developed in prior work which have
yet to be simultaneously disentangled. This paper examines the degree to which believing that events occurred
(autobiographical belief), recollecting events (recollection),
and appraising the accuracy of recollections (belief in accuracy) are distinct components of remembering.
Before discussing these concepts, we emphasize that
our focus is not on the objective accuracy of memories.
With autobiographical remembering, the objective details
of events as originally experienced are not available for
verification in the same way that an investigator can
ascertain the accuracy of recall of a word presented on a
list earlier in an experimental session. Even in cases where
photographs or diaries exist to corroborate past events,
memories remain subjective appraisals that are based on
338
A. Scoboria et al. / Cognition 136 (2015) 337–349
the information available at the time of remembering.
When external evidence challenges a memory, an individual may choose to doubt the evidence as readily as changing his or her belief in the accuracy of the memory.
The availability of ‘objective’ information is only part of
the problem of assessing accuracy in autobiographical
memory. Real-world events include a staggering amount
of multimodal information experienced over relatively
lengthy time spans. To expect complete and accurate
retrieval of all of this information seems an unreasonable
standard for accuracy. Even if one were theoretically able
to remember all of the details of a prior event, how this
information is reported becomes a problem. There are systematic influences of linguistic and narrative conventions
on free recall, and there are limitations of expression for
perceptual and emotional experiences. Characteristics of
the retrieval environment influence the type and amount
of information that is reported, such that reports about
the same event made in different contexts can vary substantially and yet be similarly accurate (Blank, 2009;
Drivdahl & Hyman, 2014). Therefore, although perhaps
odd from the perspective of traditional memory investigators, the study of autobiographical remembering can be
advanced with little reference to objective accuracy.
Our work builds on the tradition that views remembering as to some degree reconstructive in nature, and that
labelling mental representations as ‘memories’ or
otherwise reflects metacognitive attributions based on
mental experience at the time of remembering (Johnson
& Raye, 1981; Neisser, 1967; Rubin, 2005). A number of
approaches share the proposition that multiple metacognitive appraisals contribute to remembering (Mazzoni &
Kirsch, 2002; Rubin, 2006; Scoboria et al., 2014). In one
of the more comprehensive efforts, Brewer (1996) defined
autobiographical memories as possessing the distinct components of vivid imagery, belief that the resulting mental
representation represents the past faithfully, and belief
that the represented events genuinely occurred.
1.1. Recollection
Reminiscence includes conscious awareness of remembering, re-experiencing of perceptual details of the event,
recognizing the spatial and temporal characteristics of
the event, and novel appraisal of the event as it influences
current emotion. This cumulative recollective experience
results in a subjective feeling of re-experiencing the past
that differentiates remembering from imagining (Tulving,
1985). A large body of work illustrates that recollective
experience (variously named episodic memory; believed
memory; recollective memory) makes key contributions
to remembering when present (Tulving, 1983; Johnson,
Hashtroudi & Lindsay, 1996; Cabeza & Moscovitch, 2013;
Yonelinas, 2002).
There is a clear distinction between the mental simulation and the recollection of events (Addis, Pan, Vu, Laiser, &
Schacter, 2009). As discussed in the work on source monitoring (Johnson, Hastroudi, & Lindsay, 1993), it is possible
to differentiate subjective experiences of remembering
from imagining, without defining remembering as being
objectively related to past events and imagining as being
independent of objective reality. Therefore, an individual
may recollect an event that did not in fact occur and yet
still identify that experience as distinct from fantasizing
about an event that is known not to have occurred.
Recollection can thus be viewed as the presence of an
episodic mental simulation accompanied by a sense of
re-experiencing.
1.2. Autobiographical belief
Autobiographical belief (also belief in occurrence), is the
belief that an event occurred to the self in the past. While
memories have long been recognized as including a sense
of genuineness (e.g., James, 1890; Lampinen & Odegard,
2006; Pillemer, 1998; Rubin, 2012; Tulving, 1983), less
attention has been directed to the degree that recollection
and autobiographical belief are dissociable. A growing
body of evidence supports such a distinction (Mazzoni &
Kirsch, 2002; Mazzoni, Scoboria, & Harvey, 2010;
Scoboria, Mazzoni, Kirsch, & Reyea, 2004). Suggesting false
events frequently results in reports that events occurred
without accompanying recollection (Bernstein, Pernat, &
Loftus, 2011; Hart & Schooler, 2006; Scoboria, Lynn,
Hessen & Fisico, 2007). Studies of nonbelieved memories
show the converse case, in which the strength of recollection exceeds that of autobiographical belief (Clark, Nash,
Fincham, & Mazzoni, 2012; Mazzoni, Nash, & Clark, 2014;
Otgaar, Scoboria, & Smeets, 2013; Scoboria & Talarico,
2013).
Scoboria et al. (2014; see also Scoboria & Talarico, 2013,
Study 3) demonstrated that recollection and autobiographical belief form distinct latent constructs, that each is
predicted by different variables, and that their correspondence varies depending on the type of event under study.
Autobiographical belief judgments are influenced by a
wider variety of processes and sources of information.
These include the presence of recollection, the plausibility
of events, and social feedback about events, to select just
three of the more prominent influences (Scoboria,
Boucher, & Mazzoni, in press; Wade, Nash, & Garry,
2014). Such observations have led to the proposition that
autobiographical belief is the summative appraisal of all
evidence available at the time that an event is remembered
(Scoboria et al., 2014).1
1.3. Belief in accuracy
The distinction between autobiographical belief and
belief in the accuracy of a recollected mental simulation
is the difference between asking ‘‘Did this event occur?’’
and ‘‘Am I remembering this event in the way that it
occurred?’’ When an event is recollected, the details of that
recollection become subject to evaluation. The default
assumption is that one would believe the contents of a
recollection. However, most individuals are open to
re-evaluating their memory from time to time if confronted with skepticism, contradictory evidence, or a need
1
See Scoboria and Talarico (2013) for discussion of the relationship
between autobiographical belief and the remember/know distinction.
A. Scoboria et al. / Cognition 136 (2015) 337–349
to reconcile discrepant information about the self
(Pasupathi, 2001; Wilson & Ross, 2003). The contents of
recollections can be revised (e.g., a yield sign transformed
into a stop sign) without influencing the overarching
appraisal that the event did occur. Discussions of the social
functions of remembering point to the possible gains realized when people achieve reasonable agreement within
groups about the details of shared past experiences, for
example increased social cohesion (Alea & Bluck, 2003;
Pillemer, 1998). Questions regarding belief in accuracy
may be thought of as potential errors of commission – misremembering details by confusing aspects of one event
with another, by incorporating post-event information, or
constructing details of an event based on schematic information. Belief in accuracy is the individual’s functional
assessment of the accuracy of what is recalled (again,
independent of what might be considered ‘‘objective’’
accuracy).
Rubin (2006) includes both recollection and belief in
accuracy appraisals in his Basic Systems model, arguing
that each reflects distinct metamemorial processes. Belief
in accuracy is the degree to which a recollected event is
appraised to correspond to what was experienced at the
time of the event, that is, the subjective sense of correspondence between the present and the past. Evidence supports
such a distinction at the measurement level within autobiographical memory (Rubin, Schrauf, & Greenberg, 2003;
Fitzgerald & Broadbridge, 2012), and some relationships
with individual difference variables have been established
(Rubin & Siegler, 2004).
In at least some ways, discussions of the monitoring and
control of memory are similar to that of belief in accuracy.
Koriat and Goldsmith’s (1996) strategic memory regulation
model proposes that the retrieval of items from memory
and the monitoring of the accuracy of retrieved information arise from distinct processes. In this approach, the
subjective accuracy of retrieved information is assessed
using confidence ratings. As such, belief in accuracy
appraisals in the context of AM and confidence ratings
for items of knowledge in the metamemory literature are
conceptual relatives.
Much of the work on accuracy monitoring is conducted
using single items from within observed events (e.g.,
Howie & Roebers, 2007; Loftus, Miller, & Burns, 1978;
Odinot & Wolters, 2006; Sauer, Brewer, & Weber, 2012),
newly learned items (e.g., Dodson & Schacter, 2002; Hart,
1967), or items of general knowledge (e.g., Ackerman &
Goldsmith, 2008; Koriat & Goldsmith, 1996). The monitoring of whole events, which are comprised of multiple features and related knowledge bound together within a
spatial/temporal frame, is presumably more complex. The
literature on memory distortion has long distinguished
between memory for details and memory for events. Early
papers on false memory formation argued that the effects
of misinformation on event details are not of the same type
as the development of robust false memories for events
(Bruck & Ceci, 1999; Loftus & Pickrell, 1995). Hyman &
Pentland (1996) argued that the fabrication of false memories requires the development of complete, emotional,
and self-involved mental representations. Thus while confidence for individual items and belief in accuracy are
339
related, the latter is a more complex summative appraisal
of the correspondence between what is recollected and
what occurred in the past.
This constellation of constructs is not inconsistent with
what we know of the neural underpinnings of autobiographical remembering. Event representations are retained
in both hippocampal and cortical regions (Moskovitch,
2012), speaking to different modes by which event representations can be subjectively experienced. Precuneus
activity has been associated with vivid visual imagery necessary to maintain recollection (Rubin & Greenberg, 2003).
Moscovitch (2008) proposes that recollection relies heavily
on the hippocampal region, whereas evaluation of the
veridicality of retrieved material is processed in other
brain regions. The engagement of the medial temporal
lobes in reconstructing and recombining details of
retrieved events (Eichenbaum, 2001) may provide the
basis for belief in accuracy judgments. Given that spontaneous retrieval may be more common in everyday AM
than deliberate search (Berntsen, 2012; Uzer, Lee, &
Brown, 2012), right prefrontal activity previously associated with memory accessibility and ‘‘retrieval mode’’
(Nyberg, Tulving, Habib, et al., 1995) may correspond to
belief in occurrence monitoring. Similarly, medial prefrontal cortex activity previously related to self-referential processing (Craik et al., 1999; St. Jacques, Conway, Lowder, &
Cabeza, 2011) has been associated with ‘‘a collection of
processes including: decision making under uncertainty,
control processes providing a ‘feeling of rightness’ and
the processing of self-referential information that combine
to monitor the veracity of autobiographical memories’’
(Gilboa, 2004, as cited by Daselaar et al., 2006 p. 224) all
of which would seem to parallel belief in occurrence
assessment as defined by Scoboria et al. (2014). Therefore,
we have good reason to believe that these three distinct
components are necessary, though not sufficient, for autobiographical remembering.
1.4. The current research
The distinction among recollection, autobiographical
belief, and belief in accuracy in the literature is not
straightforward. As discussed, recollection has been a
defining feature of AM from the earliest theoretical work.
However, the term ‘belief’ is used loosely in the literature.
In some formulations, belief is associated with the absence
of memory (Johnson, Raye, Mitchell, & Ankudowich, 2012).
What is measured in some work more closely resembles
what we describe here as belief in accuracy, but it is often
discussed in terms more closely resembling belief in occurrence. These two types of belief are conflated in much of
the preceding research because both spontaneous generation and laboratory cueing of events typically elicits vividly
recollected events that are believed to have occurred and
which are viewed to be accurate (Scoboria & Talarico,
2013). In other words, autobiographical belief and belief
in accuracy appear to be synonymous because research
often elicits memories for which both co-occur at high levels. Furthermore, everyday experience of AM assumes
belief in occurrence. Perhaps because of this inconsistency,
earlier work on dissociating recollection and belief has
340
A. Scoboria et al. / Cognition 136 (2015) 337–349
demonstrated greater reliability in describing recollection
than ‘belief’ (Fitzgerald & Broadbridge, 2013; Rubin et al.,
2003).
Research as to whether the three concepts are empirically dissociable is needed. If belief in occurrence and belief
in accuracy represent distinct facets of remembering, presumably the concepts can be measured simultaneously
and their relationship estimated. Below we report two
studies that test if recollection, autobiographical belief,
and belief in accuracy can be modeled concurrently. In
Study 1, we used a cueing method to elicit multiple events
for which these constructs were rated. Structural equation
modelling techniques were employed to examine whether
responses were best explained by the three theorized
latent variables. In Study 2, we validated the resulting
model using confirmatory modelling techniques.
All procedures described below received clearance from
the University of Windsor Research Ethics Board and the
IRB at Lafayette College.
2. Study 1 Method
2.1. Participants
The participants were 299 students (79% female;
Mage = 22.34, SD = 4.55, range 18–50) at the University of
Windsor who received course credit.
in a pilot study, in which 10 participants selected one autobiographical memory and spoke aloud, commented on
item clarity, and described what they thought each item
was asking them to rate. We selected seven items to
include in the study: confidence that the memory is accurate, proportion of the memory that is accurate, degree to
which the memory is distorted, doubts as to accuracy of
the memory, whether details are missing from the memory, whether the memory has changed over time, and
whether the memory comes to mind in pieces. Full text
of all items contributing to the latent variables can be
found in Appendix A.
2.2.4. Additional memory characteristics
Additional items taken from preceding work served as
covariates with the theorized latent constructs (Johnson,
Foley, Suengas & Raye, 1988; Rubin, Schrauf & Greenberg,
2001; D’Argembeau & Van der Linden, 2004). Three indicators previously associated with recollection included perceptual detail, re-experiencing, and mental time travel. A
single item assessed the personal plausibility of events,
as plausibility has previously been associated with autobiographical belief. Additional items associated with autobiographical remembering in preceding work included
spatial detail, narrative coherence, susceptibility to persuasion, rehearsal, and emotion (intensity, positive affect,
negative affect). All items were rated using seven point
Likert-style scales.
2.2. Measures
2.3. Procedure
2.2.1. Belief in occurrence (autobiographical belief)
Belief in the occurrence of events was measured using
the three item scale from Scoboria et al. (2014). The items
query likelihood of occurrence, strength of belief that the
event occurred, and the event as having truly occurred.
The first item was derived from the ABMQ (Scoboria
et al., 2004), and is answered using an 8-point scale, and
the second and third items are answered using 7-point
scales.
2.2.2. Recollection
Recollection of events was measured using the three
item scale from Scoboria et al. (2014). The items query
remembering, strength of memory, and remember vs.
knowing. The first item is from the ABMQ (Scoboria et al.,
2004), and is answered using an 8-point scale. The third
item originated from Rubin, Schrauf & Greenberg (2001).
The second and third questions are answered using 7-point
scales.
2.2.3. Potential predictors of belief in accuracy
Seven items developed in pilot work were included as
potential indicators of belief in accuracy. To develop the
items, we started from the items that Rubin et al. (2003)
used to assess their conceptualization of ‘belief’. We then
adapted the items and generated items that we though
matched the conceptualization of belief in accuracy
described above, with the goal of producing items that
clearly tapped a single concept. All items were rated on
7-point scales. The resulting 10 items were administered
We used the procedure from Scoboria and Talarico
(2013) to cue participants to recall five events. Participants
read a brief introduction, and were then cued to provide a
brief 3–4 word prompt sufficient to remind them of each
event later in the study for five separate autobiographical
events from age 6–12. This age range was selected because
it produced the greatest variability in autobiographical
belief and recollection ratings in their studies. After this
cueing, participants were provided with the prompts in
random order and they rated each of the five events using
the autobiographical belief, recollection, belief in accuracy,
and memory characteristic items described above. Important to note is that all events are selected by the participants prior to making the ratings, thus knowledge of the
purpose of the research did not bias event selection.
2.4. Modelling approach
All structural equation models were tested using EQS
Version 6.1, using robust estimation (to account for some
skewness in some variables) for chi-square, fit indices,
and standard errors. We additionally employed bootstrapping of 1000 samples to further assess the stability of
model parameters. We used the recommendations by Jackson, Gillaspy and Purc-Stephenson (2009) to assess model
fit, and report chi-square, CFI and RMSEA indices for
assessing the absolute fit of models to the data, and the
AIC index when comparing hierarchically nested models.
Bias corrected confidence intervals for path coefficients
A. Scoboria et al. / Cognition 136 (2015) 337–349
and correlations based on bootstrapping of 1000 samples
are reported throughout the results.
The modelling in Study 1 was conducted in three stages.
First, the suitability of treating the potential belief in accuracy items as a single latent variable was explored, and
items were selected for inclusion in the models. Second,
a series of structural models of different complexity were
fit to the data to identify the best fitting model. The purpose of this stage is to examine the assertion that three
latent variables (recollection, belief in occurrence, belief
in accuracy) are required to account for variability in ratings, rather than some smaller number of latent variables.
Third, the covariates were added to the final model identified in the prior step to assess their relationship with the
LVs. We estimated path coefficients between the predictors and LVs (treating the covariates as predictors of the
LVs), and also estimated covariance between the predictors
and LVs. This stage examined two manners in which the
predictor variables related to the LVs that resulted from
the second stage.
341
We fit a series of structural equation models to test the
prediction that separate recollection, belief in occurrence,
and belief in accuracy latent variables underlie decisions
about autobiographical events. In Model 1, all 10 items
were loaded onto a single LV; the fit of this model was
expected to be poor. In Model 2, the three recollection
items were loaded onto a recollection LV and the remaining seven items onto a general ‘belief’ LV. This model
addresses whether belief in occurrence and belief in accuracy are conceptually redundant and reflect similar underlying processes. In Model 3, the three autobiographical
belief items were loaded onto one LV, and the other seven
items onto a second LV. This model addresses whether the
belief in accuracy and recollection items tap redundant
processes. Because we theorize that belief in accuracy
indexes monitoring processes that contribute to recollection, the correlation between belief in accuracy and recollection is expected to be high. Hence the need to examine
whether separate LVs are required. In Model 4, the three
recollection and three autobiographical belief items were
loaded onto separate LVs, and the four new items onto a
third LV. This model addresses the question if recollection,
belief in accuracy and belief in occurrence are distinct
aspects of remembering.
Table 1 provides fit indices for the models, and item
loadings are in Table 2. Per all fit indices, Model 4 (three
factors) fit the data well and notably better than the other
models. Thus the anticipated recollection, autobiographical
belief, and belief in accuracy (four items) latent variables
emerged. Belief in occurrence was estimated to correlate
with recollection at .56 [95% CI, .44, .68] and with belief
in accuracy at .45 [.33, .57]. Recollection correlated with
belief in accuracy at .85 [.79, .90].
We next introduced the covariates into Model 4.3 Single
item covariates included perceptual detail, spatial detail,
negative emotion, and susceptibility to persuasion. The
reliving and mental time travel items were loaded onto a
‘re-experiencing’ latent variable. In one model, pathways
from each covariate to each LV were estimated. In a second
model, correlations between each predictor and LV were
estimated. Based on prior findings, we expected that personal plausibility would be related to autobiographical belief
more strongly than the other LVs, and that perceptual detail
and re-experiencing would be more strongly associated with
recollection. Due to the theorized close relationship between
recollection and belief in accuracy, we anticipated that some
of the same variables would be associated with both LVs. All
of the covariates were permitted to correlate, because our
focus was to assess the relationship between the covariates
and the LVs; it is for this reason that the fit of the model
when including the covariates is expected to be slightly
poorer than the best fitting model above.
The fit of the final model with the predictors added
(Model 4a in Table 1) was good. The path coefficients and
estimated correlations are provided in Table 2. Regarding
the path coefficients, autobiographical belief (Cronbach’s
a = .85) was predicted strongly by personal plausibility,
and moderately by spatial detail and susceptibility to persuasion (negatively). Recollection (a = .88) was predicted
2
The potential presence of a fourth factor, cohesion among details in the
event representation, in the data for both studies is described in
supplement.
3
The three emotion items and the rehearsal item showed no notable
relationships with any of the LVs, and were dropped from the analyses.
3. Study 1 results
3.1. Data screening and exploration
A small number (<2%) of randomly distributed missing
values were imputed using the expectation maximization
procedure in SPSS v21.0. Each variable was averaged across
the five events for analysis, resulting in a single score for
each item for each participant. Examination of multivariate
outliers led to the removal of 14 extreme cases. Examination of item distributions indicated that all items were
normally distributed, excepting (as expected) that the
autobiographical belief items tended to be rated toward
the scale ceiling. Scoboria et al. (2014) reported distinct
autobiographical belief and recollection factors under similar conditions, and the robust estimation and bootstrapping techniques used to fit models directly addressed this
non-normality in item distributions.
Prior to fitting the hypothesized LVs to the data, we
examined correlations between the seven potential belief
in accuracy items. Due to finding a range of magnitude in
the relationships, we conducted an exploratory factor analysis to determine if item covariance was better explained
by one or multiple factors, using the principal factor
extraction method. Based on the eigenvalue and scree
methods, two factors explained 77.42% of item variance.
The first factor contained four items with loadings above
.70 and no cross-loading on the second factor (proportion
accurate, confidence, distorted, doubts) which reflected
our conceptualization of belief in occurrence. We used
these four items to model belief in occurrence below.2
3.2. Structural equation models
342
A. Scoboria et al. / Cognition 136 (2015) 337–349
Table 1
Study 1, structural model fit for varying levels of model complexity.
Model
Model
Model
Model
Model
1, 1 LV
2, 2 LVs (recollection; Belief in occurrence plus 4 items)
3, 2 LVs (belief in occurrence; recollection plus 4 items)
4, 3 LVs (recollection, belief in occurrence, belief in accuracy)
4a, 3 LVs with additional predictors (all paths included)a
df
v2
CFI
SRMR
RMSEA [90% CI]
AICb
55
34
34
23
85
492.06
414.17
169.18
59.20
216.31
.78
.82
.92
.99
.97
.04
.04
.04
.02
.03
.21
.20
.13
.06
.07
532.06
456.17
243.05
111.20
[.20,
[.18,
[.11,
[.04,
[.06,
.23]
.22]
.15]
.08]
.09]
Models 1 through 4 test whether reports are best described by one, two or three latent variables.
a
Model 4a is expected to not fit the data as well as Model 4, as the purpose of this model is to estimate the relationships between predictors and the LVs
and not to account for all variance in the predictors.
b
AIC is reported for models that include the same observed variables. Model 4a uses different variables and the respective fit indices cannot be directly
contrasted with the other models.
4. Study 1 summary
Table 2
Study 1, loadings of indicators on latent variables.
Item
LV
Estimate
95% CI
Memory
Memory strength
Remember/know
Belief
Belief strength
True event
Doubt
Proportion accurate
Confidence
Distorted
Recc
Recc
Recc
B.occ
B.occ
B.occ
B.acc
B.acc
B.acc
B.acc
.84
.86
.84
.81
.84
.80
.89
.92
.82
.85
.77
.80
.77
.71
.75
.67
.85
.90
.78
.78
.89
.90
.88
.87
.91
.89
.92
.95
.86
.90
Loadings of the items on the latent variables (LVs) by study. B.occ – Belief
in occurrence; Recc – Recollection; B.acc – Belief in accuracy. Bias corrected 95% confidence intervals are based on 1000 bootstrapped samples.
strongly by perceptual detail; moderately by re-experiencing and spatial detail; and weakly by spatial detail, plausibility and persuasion (negatively). Belief in accuracy
(a = .92) was predicted strongly by perceptual detail; moderately by narrative coherence; and weakly by persuasion
(negatively). The estimated correlations between the
covariates and the LVs mirrored this pattern. Plausibility
and belief in occurrence shared substantial variance,
whereas plausibility was not notably related to the other
LVs. Both perceptual detail and re-experiencing shared
substantial variance with recollection and belief in accuracy, but not with belief in occurrence (see Table 3).
This study supports the assertion that belief in occurrence, recollection, and belief in accuracy reflect distinct
components of the experience of remembering. Item variance was best explained by three distinct latent variables.
The inclusion of the predictors in the final model confirmed a strong relationship between plausibility and autobiographical belief, but not with recollection or belief in
accuracy. Perceptual detail strongly predicted recollection
and belief in accuracy, consistent with conceptualizations
of perceptual imagery as central to episodic recollection.
Re-experiencing was uniquely associated with the recollection LV, consistent with the notion that the experience
of imagery for memories is differentiated from imagery
for other mental representations in part by the presence
of a re-experiencing signal. Spatial detail was related to
belief in occurrence and recollection, but not belief in
accuracy which is not consistent with prior work (Rubin,
Schrauf & Greenberg, 2001).
To assess the generality of the findings, we conducted a
second study in a larger, more representative sample.
5. Study 2 method
5.1. Participants
We recruited a sample of Mechanical Turk workers.
Turk is an online work-for-hire site where task requests
Table 3
Study 1, standardized path coefficients and correlations between covariates and latent variables.
Covariate
Latent variable
Recollection
Perceptual detail
Spatial detail
Re-experiencingc
Personal plausibility
Persuade
Narrative coherence
Belief in occurrence
Belief in accuracy
Path coefficienta
R2b
Path coefficient
R2
Path coefficient
R2
.43 [.28, .56]
.16 [.07, .27]
.22 [.06, .41]
.08 [.01, .14]
.11 [ .18, .04]
.16 [.03, .28]
.78
.47
.71
.06
.23
.57
.07 [ .16, .03]
.22 [.10, .36]
.02 [ .24, .23]
.45 [.32, .58]
.30 [ .42, .19]
.12 [ .04, .26]
.11
.21
.10
.31
.20
.09
.52 [.35, .68]
.06 [ .15, .03]
.02 [ .15, .20]
.03 [ .04, .09]
.19 [ .27, .12]
.30 [.18, .42]
.66
.24
.51
.02
.24
.54
Note: Path coefficients are standardized regression weights. Weights for which the path coefficient is estimated to not overlap with zero are in bold. 95%
confidence intervals in brackets are based on 1000 bootstrapped samples.
a
Model with standardized path coefficients between covariates and the LVs; covariates treated as predictors of the LVs.
b
Model with correlations estimated between covariates and the LVs.
c
Re-experiencing was entered into the model as a latent variable with two indicators (reliving, mental time travel).
A. Scoboria et al. / Cognition 136 (2015) 337–349
can be posted for workers to complete. Turk workers are
required to produce quality work to remain eligible for
subsequent tasks, hence they tend to perform tasks conscientiously. Various studies have documented that data
gathered via Turk is of good quality (e.g., Buhrmester,
Kwang, & Gosling, 2011; Shapiro, Chandler, & Mueller,
2013), providing that the research design is suitable to
online administration and validity checks are incorporated.
In light of the brevity of the procedure, we paid all workers
who provided at least one data point.
A total of 1026 respondents completed the 10 min. survey and received $0.75 USD. The characteristics of the
complete sample were: 47.3% female, 50.2% male, 2.5%
genderqueer or no response; Mage = 32.50, SD = 11.25,
range 18–72; self-identified ethnicity, 9.4% Asian, 5.8%
black, 4.9% Hispanic, 1.8% mixed, 0.4% Native American,
73.9% white; 94.2% USA resident. See below for data
screening and verification procedures and final numbers
included in analyses.
5.2. Measures
The items were the same as in Study 1, with the following exceptions (see Appendix A for items). A new item that
assessed the completeness of the memory representation
was added for exploratory purposes (see the Supplement
for more information). To further situate the measurement
of belief in accuracy in relation to Rubin’s work, two additional items from Rubin et al. (2001) were included (belief
in memory; testify). To keep the number of ratings low due
to time constraints, the predictors were: personal plausibility, perceptual detail, spatial detail, setting (new in
Study 2), reliving, and mental time travel, bringing the
total number of items to 22.
5.3. Procedure
Participants followed a link to a secure online survey.
They first recalled any childhood event using the same
cue instruction as in Study 1 and rated the event. They then
recalled an event about which they were uncertain, with
no age specified. The instructions were:
Please think about an event for which you are uncertain
about what you remember. This might mean that you
are not certain about all of the details that you recall,
that something seems to be missing from what you
remember, that something is just not quite right about
the memory, or you may not have a memory for the
event at all.
This prompt was intentionally broad to encourage
retrieval of a variety of uncertain events. Participants rated
the properties of the memory and then described why they
were uncertain about the event before providing demographic information.
343
of 27 individuals completed the survey in less than
2 min. and/or provided inconsistent responses that did
not pass all validity checks and were removed. Examination of multivariate outliers for the childhood event led
to the removal of 11 cases, resulting in a total of 988 usable
events. The reasons provided for being uncertain about the
second event were reviewed, and 57 cases were dropped
because the respondent wrote that they were not uncertain or provided no explanation. Another nine multivariate
outliers were removed, leaving a total of 933 usable cases
for the uncertain event. Thus 933 individuals provided
valid responses for both events, and were used in the analyses described below.4
5.5. Modelling approach
Modelling was conducted using the same estimation
and fit indices as in Study 1. We adopted the approach
for modelling events used by Scoboria et al. (2014, Study
2), and modelled both events simultaneously. As our primary goal was to confirm the best fitting structural model
(Model 4) from Study 1, we fit that exact model to the new
dataset. Second, we explored adding the additional indicators to the belief in accuracy LV. This resulted in the final
structural model. Third, we added the predictors to the
final model.
6. Study 2 results
The first model assessed whether the final structural
model (Model 4) from Study 1 fit the data for both the
childhood and uncertain events, when modelling the three
latent variables for the two events simultaneously. Thus a
total of six LVs were estimated, with each of the three constructs (recollection, belief in occurrence, belief in accuracy) modelled twice. Correlations between the LVs
within each event were also estimated, and repeated items
were permitted to covary to account for variance due to
repeated measures. The model fit the data well, v2
(141) = 360.21, CFI = .985, SRMR = .052, RMSEA = .040
(.035, .046).
In the second model, we explored adding the two additional indicators of belief in occurrence. Adding the ‘belief
in memory’ item led to a notable decrease in model fit, and
the item did not load highly on the belief in accuracy LV.
This finding is consistent with Scoboria et al. (2014), and
is likely because this item refers to multiple concepts;
the item was dropped from further analysis. Inclusion of
the testify item on the belief in accuracy LV did not notably
affect model fit: v2 (179) = 467.79, CFI = .982, SRMR = .053,
RMSEA = .041 (.037, .046). The item was retained because
this item better incorporates Rubin and colleagues’ preceding work into the current framework. The new ‘complete’
item did not load on these factors and was not retained.
See Fig. 1 for a graphical depiction of this final structural
5.4. Data screening
Time to complete the survey (Mtime = 8 m 30 s) and
three embedded validity checks were examined. A total
4
Modelling all 988 participants with a valid childhood event did not
reveal any notable differences in estimated item loadings, LV correlations,
or path coefficients, compared to the models that used 933 participants.
344
A. Scoboria et al. / Cognition 136 (2015) 337–349
model, including item loadings and correlations between
the LVs.
Having finalized the stability of the structural model,
we added the covariates to the model. Single item covariates in this model included perceptual detail and personal
plausibility. The reliving and mental time travel items
were loaded onto a re-experiencing latent variable
(a = .84). The spatial detail and setting items were loaded
onto a spatial latent variable (a = .82). The covariates were
allowed to correlate in the model. See Table 4 for the
resulting estimates. The relationships between the predictors and LVs were similar to Study 1. First looking at the
path coefficients, autobiographical belief was strongly predicted by plausibility, and plausibility did not predict the
other LVs. The recollection and belief in accuracy LVs were
predicted strongly by perceptual detail, and moderately by
re-experiencing. In this study setting predicted all three
LVs to a similar, moderate degree. The estimated correlations between the covariates and the LVs mirrored this
pattern. Personal plausibility shared substantial variance
with belief in occurrence, but not the other LVs, for both
events.
There was one notable difference in this pattern
between the two events. Personal plausibility was a stronger predictor of (coefficient .68 vs. .42) and shared more
variance with (R2 .53 vs. .24) autobiographical belief for
the uncertain than for the childhood event. This suggests
that people rely on plausibility to a greater extent when
not certain about autobiographical events.
The uncertain event was included to provide another
type of memory to assess the model fit, and to provide
another method of examining any implications of nearceiling effects for the indicators of autobiographical belief.
Fig. 2 presents mean levels for the constructs across both
events, confirming that the ratings were lower for the
uncertain event. The invariance in measurement between
the two events also indicates that the exploratory model
was not affected by high autobiographical belief ratings
for the earlier events. The fact that cueing for an uncertain
event results in obtaining events with overall lower autobiographical belief ratings is important, in light of concerns
that cues often result in the retrieval of events that are
associated with high levels of both autobiographical belief
and recollection (Scoboria & Talarico, 2013).
Fig. 1. Study 2, final structural model. Both of the events are modelled simultaneously. Bias corrected 95% confidence intervals are reported for item loading
and latent variable correlations. B.occ: Belief in occurrence LV; Recc: Recollection LV; B.acc: Belief in accuracy LV.
345
A. Scoboria et al. / Cognition 136 (2015) 337–349
Table 4
Study 2, standardized path coefficients when predicting the latent variables, by event.
Event
Covariate
Latent variable
Recollection
Belief in occurrence
Belief in accuracy
Path coefficienta
R2b
Path coefficient
R2
Path coefficient
R2
Childhood event
Perceptual detail
Settingc
Re-experiencingd
Personal plausibility
.38
.25
.20
.05
[.26, .51]
[.09, .41]
[.12, .30]
[ .02, .11]
.32
.11
.09
.05
.16 [.05, .30]
.22 [.05, .39]
.14 [ .22, .07]
.42 [.32, .53]
.11
.01
.00
.24
.38 [.25, .48]
.26 [.13, .40]
.21 [.12, .29]
.06 [ .01, .12]
.34
.11
.09
.05
Uncertain event
Perceptual detail
Settingc
Re-experiencingd
Personal plausibility
.45
.26
.15
.02
[.34, .55]
[.14, .38]
[.05, .24]
[ .03, .08]
.39
.10
.09
.04
.09 [ .02, .17]
.16 [.05, .28]
.12 [ .21, .05]
.68 [.62, .73]
.09
.00
.00
.53
.37
.35
.10
.10
.34
.13
.07
.09
[.27,
[.24,
[.02,
[.04,
.45]
.47]
.19]
.16]
Note: Estimated path coefficients from the SEM model simultaneously predicting the three latent variables for each of the two events.
a
Model with standardized path coefficients between covariates and the LVs; covariates treated as predictors of the LVs.
b
Model with correlations estimated between covariates and the LVs.
c
Setting was entered into the model as a latent variable with two indicators (space, setting).
d
Re-experiencing was entered into the model as a latent variable with two indicators (reliving, mental time travel). 95% confidence intervals in brackets
are based on 1000 bootstrapped samples. Path coefficients that do not include zero in the interval are in bold.
7. General discussion
This research demonstrates that recollection, autobiographical belief, and belief in the accuracy of recollective
mental representations are distinct metamemory appraisals that contribute to the remembering of autobiographical
events. While existing theories agree that recollection is a
central component of remembering, this work indicates
that theories emphasizing appraisals of accuracy (Rubin,
2005; Rubin, 2012) and appraisals of occurrence (Mazzoni
& Kirsch, 2002; Scoboria et al., 2004; Scoboria et al., 2014)
are complementary. These studies validate that a number
of the distinct features of autobiographical memory (AM)
articulated by Brewer (1996) are independent.
This research adds to the body of work which shows
that belief in the occurrence of events is distinct from the
recollection of events. Similar to preceding studies, their
indicators were different, estimated correlations between
the two were no greater than modest, and a number of
their predictors were distinct. Recollection indexes the
appraisal that a current mental experience is reflective of
a prior episode of experience. Recollection has long been
described as having the key components of perceptual
imagery and an associated sense of re-experiencing
(James, 1890/1950; Tulving, 1985). Evidence has emerged
which supports the assertion that recollection is
distinguished from other forms of mental simulation
(e.g., simulating future events) by the presence of the sense
of re-experiencing the past (Addis et al., 2009). These studies provide further evidence that perceptual imagery and
re-experiencing are central to recollection.
Belief in occurrence is an appraisal of the truth status of
events, regardless of the degree to which recollection is
present. Scoboria et al. (2014) argue that recollection is a
key contributor to autobiographical belief appraisals, such
that when recollection is strong, autobiographical belief is
often rapidly inferred. But sometimes strong recollection is
discrepant with other information about events, resulting
in a weighing of evidence to arrive at decisions about the
truth status of events. Event plausibility is consistently
associated with autobiographical belief appraisals in this
and preceding work. Thus autobiographical belief judgments are contingent on viewing events as ontologically
possible, both in terms of what is possible in the world
and what is viewed as possible for oneself in the past.
Event plausibility plays an important gating function by
preventing the processing of events that are ‘impossible’
(Odegard & Lampinen, 2006; Scoboria, Mazzoni, Jarry, &
Shapero, 2012). A new finding in the second study is that
plausibility is more strongly predictive of autobiographical
belief when memories about which individuals are somehow uncertain are assessed. In other words, when recollection is experienced as unreliable individuals rely on other
sources of information to appraise the truth status of
events.
Newly established here is that belief in occurrence and
belief in accuracy are likewise distinct. Each emerged as a
distinct latent variable in the confirmatory modelling
process, correlations between the two were moderate in
magnitude, and the covariates differentiated the two. Such
a distinction aligns well with the view that autobiographical belief judgments are sometimes made in the presence
of strong and compelling episodic mental imagery, at other
times in the presence of lower quality episodic imagery,
and still at other times in the absence of any mental imagery. Belief in accuracy judgments are appraisals of the
quality of the episodic image – to what extent are the
details within the representation associated with the event
an accurate reflection of a prior state of affairs. This distinction leads to the conclusion that it is possible to edit the
details within a memory without altering the overarching
autobiographical belief appraisal. For example, two people
might agree that an event occurred at a particular time and
in a particular place, but disagree as to some of the withinevent details such as who did what. It is also possible to
reattribute the source of an event without any alteration
to the view that the event is being recalled accurately.
Examples of such cases have been discuss in the literature
on nonbelieved memories (Scoboria et al., in press). A person may describe a vivid memory of an event, in response
346
A. Scoboria et al. / Cognition 136 (2015) 337–349
Fig. 2. Study 2, average ratings for each of the latent variables by memory
cue. Bars show 95% confidence intervals on the means.
to which another person states that the event did not happened to them. Sometimes the other person is correct, and
the rememberer also decides that the other person is correct and relinquishes autobiographical belief for the memory. However, at the same time the other person may agree
that the mental representation is a quite accurate description of what occurred in the event. Hence the event is
recalled with good accuracy, but is associated with a
source monitoring error as to whom the event in fact
occurred. For a recent theoretical view on the relationship
between belief in occurrence and source monitoring attributions, see Nash, Wheeler, and Hope (in press).
The fact that predictors related to imagery loaded on
the belief in accuracy LV and the high correlation between
the belief in accuracy and recollection LVs further support
the notion that belief in accuracy reflects monitoring of the
accuracy of recollected images, as proposed by Rubin
(2005). Recollection appraisals arise out of the results of
the processes reflected in belief in accuracy appraisals. In
the more general terms proposed by Nelson and Narens
(1994), metacognitive monitoring processes (belief in
accuracy) serve to evaluate and control the output of other
cognitive systems (recollection). This points to the value of
pursuing further linkages between the study of episodic
and autobiographical memory and the more general memory monitoring literatures. To accomplish this, further theorizing and empirical work as to the monitoring of whole
events vs. details within events is needed. The relationship
between confidence in individual event details (measured
in much of the work on memory and metacognition) and
overarching accuracy appraisals for whole events are
unknown at this time. If automatic appraisals of autobiographical memory are global and the default assumption
is to believe the contents of memories, then drawing attention to event components is likely to decrease belief in
accuracy judgments. As each detail is met with scepticism,
the accumulated belief value can only diminish.
This distinction between appraisals for events and
items within events raises new questions as to how findings in the literatures on misinformation, interviewing
about witnessed events, and questioning about details
within scenes depend on the interviewee being completely
certain that the event itself occurred. In the typical misinformation study, there is little doubt for participants that
the event has been witnessed. Thus the focus of such studies is on how misinformation influences the recall of
details within events. Appraisals for the details of events
for which autobiographical belief is high may be quite different than memory for the details of events for which
autobiographical belief is lower. For example, one might
expect differences in recall between someone who knows
that they are witnessing a crime as it occurs vs. another
person who was in the vicinity and is later uncertain if they
saw the crime. Studies that are directed at memory for
details (e.g., items on lists, contents of rooms, details in
scenes) may have more to do with micro-level, withinevent processes than they do with overarching eventrelated processes. As Scoboria et al. (2014) note, belief in
occurrence and belief in accuracy are likely confounded
when single items are the target of study. Research methods that examine single items in isolation may tell us little
about the recollection of whole events or the relationships
among details within events.
Two notes as to the inclusion of predictors of the latent
variables in this model are warranted. First, a number of
the items that we use as predictors of recollection and
belief in accuracy are assumed to be measures of these
constructs in other work (e.g., Fitzgerald & Broadbridge,
2012). We treat them as predictors because this approach
more closely mirrors Rubin’s (2006) argument that distinct
component processes contribute to the overarching metacognitive judgments that are modelled in this research. For
example, the strength of visual imagery is one of a number
of inputs that has a direct influence on appraisals of recollection. Second, we emphasize that the goal of this work
was to establish whether memory reports included distinct
belief in occurrence, recollection, and belief in accuracy
components. The main finding of these studies arises from
the structural modelling which shows the measurement
distinction and estimates the correlation between the
resulting latent variables. The predictors included in this
work were selected to partly illuminate the distinction,
and are not intended to be comprehensive. Further work
is needed to flesh out the model in order to identify how
different theorized constructs relate to the constructs identified herein. More comprehensive measurement of the
mental simulation of events clearly requires attention.
Notions of the coherence of narratives and remembering
as storytelling will need to be incorporated (Rubin, 2006).
Issues of identity centrality and the personal impact of
events need to be considered (Berntsen & Rubin, 2006;
Brown, Hansen, Lee, Vanderveen, & Conrad, 2012), particularly in relation to autobiographical belief processing. The
widely documented influences of a variety of social processes on these constructs require elucidation (Drivdahl
& Hyman, 2014; Hirst & Echterhoff, 2012; Pillemer,
1998). The efforts to predict the key latent variables demonstrated in this work have only just begun, and we look
forward to future work that examines additional predictors
of the factors discussed here.
A possible process for remembering an autobiographical memory is to assess the plausibility of a cue, arrive at
A. Scoboria et al. / Cognition 136 (2015) 337–349
a decision regarding belief in the occurrence of the event,
engage in further efforts to retrieve the details of that
event, formulate a perceptual representation of the event,
and then evaluate the accuracy of this representation.
The sequence of these steps need not be in a fixed order,
and may occur in parallel. What seems clear is that for
an event to be labeled as recollected, sufficient information
must be present to produce a representation that is viewed
to be sufficiently accurate. What is becoming equally clear
is that decisions about event occurrence happen in the
absence of recollection, and in some cases, in contradiction
to recollection. We look forward to future theorizing in
which the parameters of decision making about the occurrence and accuracy of autobiographical events are more
fully articulated.
347
the event as a neutral observer would report it? (1
100% distorted; 7 100% accurate).
4. Doubts. Do you have any doubts about the accuracy of
your memory for this event? (1 A great deal of doubts;
7 no doubts whatsoever).
5. Testify.5 Would you be confident enough in your memory
of the event to testify in a court of law?
(1 Not at all; 3 vaugely; 5 distinctly; 7 as much as any
event).
Appendix B. Supplementary material
Supplementary data associated with this article can be
found, in the online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.cognition.2014.11.028.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by a Natural Science and
Engineering Research Council of Canada Discovery Grant
to the first author. We extend our thanks to Kyrsten
Grimes and Cara Austen for assistance with data collection.
Appendix A
Items contributing to the latent variables.
Autobiographical belief/belief in occurrence items:
1. ABMQ belief. How likely is it that you personally did in
fact experience this event? (1 Definitely did not happen; 8 definitely happened).
2. Belief strength. How strong is your belief that this event
actually occurred? (1 No belief; 3 weak belief; 5 Moderate belief; 7 strong belief).
3. True event. It is true that this event occurred. (1 Not at
all true; 7 completely true).
Recollection items
1. ABMQ memory. Do you actually remember experiencing
the event? (1 No memory at all; 8 clear and complete
memory).
2. Memory strength. How strong is your memory for this
event; (1 No memory; 3 weak memory; 5 moderate
memory; 7 strong memory).
3. Remember/know. As you think about this event, can you
actually remember it rather than just knowing that it
occurred? (1 Not at all; 3 vaguely; 5 distinctly; 7 more
than any event).
Belief in accuracy items
1. Confidence. How confident are you that your memory
for this event is accurate? (1 Not at all confident; 7
completely confident).
2. Proportion accurate. What proportion of your memory
for this event is accurate? (1 Not at all accurate; 7
100% accurate).
3. Distorted. To what extent is your memory of the event
distorted, rather than being an accurate reflection of
References
Ackerman, R., & Goldsmith, M. (2008). Control over grain size in memory
reporting – With and without satisfying knowledge. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34,
1224–1245. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012938.
Addis, D. R., Pan, L., Vu, M. A., Laiser, N., & Schacter, D. L. (2009).
Constructive episodic simulation of the future and the past: Distinct
subsystems of a core brain network mediate imagining and
remembering. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2222–2238. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.026.
Alea, N., & Bluck, S. (2003). Why are you telling me that? A conceptual
model of the social function of autobiographical memory. Memory, 11,
165–178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/741938207.
Baddeley, A. (2012). Reflections on autobiographical memory. In D.
Berntsen & D. C. Rubin (Eds.), Understanding autobiographical memory:
Theories and approaches (pp. 70–88). Cambridge: University Press.
Bernstein, D. M., Pernat, N. L. M., & Loftus, E. (2011). The false memory
diet: False memories alter food preferences. In V. R. Preedy, R. R.
Watson, & C. R. Martin (Eds.), Handbook of behaviour, food, and
nutrition (pp. 1645–1663). Ney York: Springer.
Berntsen,
D.
(2012).
Spontaneous
recollections:
Involuntary
autobiographical memories are a basic mode of remembering. In D.
Berntsen & D. C. Rubin (Eds.), Understanding autobiographical memory:
Theories and approaches (pp. 290–310). Cambridge: University Press.
Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2006). The centrality of event scale: A
measure of integrating a trauma into one’s identity and its relation to
post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms. Behavior Research and
Therapy, 44, 219–231.
Blank, H. (2009). Remembering: A theoretical interface between memory
and social psychology. Social Psychology, 40, 164–175. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335.40.3.164.
Brewer, W. F. (1996). What is collective memory. In D. C. Rubin (Ed.),
Remembering our past: Studies in autobiographical memory (pp. 66–96).
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Brown, N. R., Hansen, T. G. B., Lee, P. J., Vanderveen, S. A., & Conrad, F. G.
(2012). Historically-defined autobiographical periods: Their origins
and implications. In D. Berntsen & D. Rubin (Eds.), Understanding
autobiographical memories: Theories and approaches. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Bruck, M., & Ceci, S. J. (1999). The suggestibility of children’s memory.
Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 419–439.
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s mechanical
Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data?
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3–5. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1177/1745691610393980.
Cabeza, R., & Moscovitch, M. (2013). Memory systems, processing modes,
and components: Functional neuroimaging evidence. Perspectives on
Psychological
Science,
8,
49–55.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
1745691612469033.
Clark, A., Nash, R. A., Fincham, G., & Mazzoni, G. (2012). Creating nonbelieved memories for recent autobiographical events. PLoS ONE, 7.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0032998.
5
Administered in Study 2 only.
348
A. Scoboria et al. / Cognition 136 (2015) 337–349
Conway, M. A., Singer, J. A., & Tagini, A. (2004). The self and
autobiographical memory: Correspondence and coherence. Social
Cognition, 22, 495–537.
Craik, F. I. M., Moroz, T. M., Moscovitch, M., Stuss, D. T., Winocur, G.,
Tulving, E., et al. (1999). In search of the self: A positron emission
tomography study. Psychological Science, 10, 26–34.
Daselaar, S. M., Fleck, M. S., Prince, S. E., & Cabeza, R. (2006). The medial
temporal lobe distinguishes old from new independent of
consciousness. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 5835–5839.
Dodson, C. S., & Schacter, D. L. (2002). When false recognition meets
metacognition: The distinctiveness heuristic. Journal of Memory and
Language, 46, 782–803. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2001.2822.
Drivdahl, S. B., & Hyman, I. E. (2014). Fluidity in autobiographical
memories: Relationship memories sampled on two occasions.
Memory,
22,
1070–1081.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
09658211.2013.866683.
Eichenbaum, H. (2001). The hippocampus and declarative memory:
Cognitive mechanisms and neural codes. Behavioural Brain Research,
127, 199–207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00365-5.
Fitzgerald, J. M., & Broadbridge, C. L. (2013). Latent constructs of the
autobiographical memory questionnaire: A recollection-belief model
of autobiographical experience. Memory, 21, 230–248. doi:
10.1080é09658211.2012.725736.
Hart, J. T. (1967). Second-try recall, recognition, and the memorymonitoring process. Journal of Educational Psychology, 58, 193–197.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0024908.
Hart, R., & Schooler, J. (2006). Increasing belief in an invasive procedure
that never happened: The role of plausibility and schematicity.
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 661–669. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
acp.1218.
Hirst, W., & Echterhoff, G. (2012). Remembering in conversations: The
social sharing and reshaping of memories. Annual Review of
Psychology, 63, 55–69.
Howie, P., & Roebers, C. M. (2007). Developmental progression in the
confidence-accuracy relationship in event recall: Insights provided by
a calibration perspective. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 871–893.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.1302.
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology, vol. 2. Oxford, England:
Dover Publications.
Johnson, M. K., Hastroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring.
Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3–28. doi: 10.1037é0033-2909.
Johnson, M. K., & Raye, C. L. (1981). Reality monitoring. Psychological
Review, 88, 76–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.88.1.67.
Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Mitchell, K. J., & Ankudowich, E. (2012). True
and false recovered memories: Toward a reconciliation of the debate.
In R. F. Belli (Ed.) (True and false recovered memories: Toward a
reconciliation of the debate. Nebraska symposium on motivation. Vol.
58, pp. 15–52). New York, NY, US: Springer Science.
Koriat, A., & Goldsmith, M. (1996). Monitoring and control processes in
the strategic regulation of memory accuracy. Behavioral and Brain
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/
Sciences,
19,
167–228.
S0140525X00042114.
Lampinen, J. M., & Odegard, T. N. (2006). Memory editing mechanisms.
Memory,
14,
649–654.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
09658210600648407.
Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic integration of
verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 19–31. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/0278-7393.4.1.19.
Loftus, E. F., & Pickrell, J. E. (1995). The formation of false memories.
Psychiatric Annals, 25, 720–725.
Mazzoni, G., & Kirsch, I. (2002). Autobiographical memories and beliefs: A
preliminary metacognitive model. In T. J. Perfect & B. L. Schwartz
(Eds.), Applied Metacognition (pp. 121–145). Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Mazzoni, G. A. L., Nash, R. A., & Clark, A. (2014). Disowned recollections:
Denying true experiences undermines belief in occurrence but not
judgments of remembering. Acta Psychologica, 145, 139–146.
Mazzoni, G., Scoboria, A., & Harvey, L. (2010). Nonbelieved memories.
Psychological Science, 21, 1334–1340. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0956797610379865.
Moscovitch, M. (2008). The hippocampus as a ‘‘Stupid’’, domain-specific
module: Implications for theories of recent and remote memory, and
of imagination. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62,
62–79.
Nash, R. A., Wheeler, R. L., & Hope, L. (in press). On the persuadability of
memory: Is changing people’s memories no more than changing their
minds? British Journal of Psychology. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
bjop.12074.
Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. East Norwalk, CT, US: AppletonCentury-Crofts.
Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1994). Why investigate metacognition? In J.
Metcalfe & A. P. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about
knowing (pp. 1–25). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Nyberg, L., Tulving, E., Habib, R., et al. (1995). Functional brain maps of
retrieval mode and recovery of episodic information. Neuroreport: An
International Journal for the Rapid Communication of Research in
Neuroscience, 7, 249–252.
Odegard, T. N., & Lampinen, J. M. (2006). Memory editing: Knowledge,
criteria, and alignment. Memory, 14, 777–787. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1080/09658210600648589.
Odinot, G., & Wolters, G. (2006). Repeated recall, retention interval and
the accuracy-confidence relation in eyewitness memory. Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 20, 973–985. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
acp.1263.
Otgaar, H., Scoboria, A., & Smeets, T. (2013). Experimentally evoking
nonbelieved memories for childhood events. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 717–730. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029668.
Pasupathi, M. (2001). The social construction of personal past and its
implications for adult development. Psychological Bulletin, 127,
651–672. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.5.651.
Pillemer, D. B. (1998). Momentous events, vivid memories. Cambridge, MA,
US: Harvard University Press.
Rubin, D. C. (2005). A basic-systems approach to autobiographical
memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 79–83.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00339.x.
Rubin, D. C. (2006). The basic-systems model of episodic memory.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 277–311. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00017.x.
Rubin, D. C. (2012). The basic systems model of autobiographical memory.
In D. Berntsen & D. C. Rubin (Eds.), Understanding autobiographical
memory: Theories and approaches (pp. 11–32). Cambridge: University
Press.
Rubin, D. C., Schrauf, R. W., & Greenberg, D. L. (2003). Belief and
recollection of autobiographical memories. Memory and Cognition,
31(887–901), 1037. doi: 103758/BF03196443.
Rubin, D. C., & Siegler, I. C. (2004). Facets of personality and the
phenomenology of autobiographical memory. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 18, 913–930. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.1038.
Sauer, J. D., Brewer, N., & Weber, N. (2012). Using confidence ratings to
identify a target among foils. Journal of Applied Research in Memory
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
and
Cognition,
1,
80–88.
j.jarmac.2012.03.003.
Scoboria, A., Boucher, C., & Mazzoni, G. (in press). Reasons for
withdrawing belief in vivid autobiographical memories. Memory.
doi: <http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2014.910530>.
Scoboria, A., Jackson, D. L., Talarico, J., Hanchowski, M., Wysman, L., &
Mazzoni, G. (2014). The role of belief in occurrence within
autobiographical memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 143, 1242–1258.
Scoboria, A., Lynn, S. J., Hessen, J., & Fisico, S. (2007). So that’s why I don’t
remember: Normalising forgetting of childhood events influences
false autobiographical beliefs but not memories. Memory, 15,
801–813. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210701685266.
Scoboria, A., Mazzoni, G., Jarry, J., & Shapero, D. (2012). Implausibility
inhibits but does not eliminate false autobiographical beliefs.
Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 259–267. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0030017.
Scoboria, A., Mazzoni, G., Kirsch, I., & Reyea, M. (2004). Plausibility and
belief in autobiographical memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18,
791–807. http://dx.doi.org/10.1005/acp.1062.
Scoboria, A., & Talarico, J. M. (2013). Indirect cueing elicits distinct types
of autobiographical event representations. Consciousness and
Cognition, 22, 1495–1509.
Shapiro, D. N., Chandler, J., & Mueller, P. A. (2013). Using mechanical
Turk to study clinical populations. Clinical Psychological Science, 1,
213–220.
St. Jacques, P. L., Conway, M. A., Lowder, M., & Cabeza, R. (2011). Watching
my mind unfold versus yours: An fMRI study using a novel camera
technology to examine the neural correlates of self-projection of self
versus other. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 1275–1284.
Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology, 26,
1–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0080017.
Uzer, T., Lee, P. J., & Brown, N. R. (2012). On the prevalence of directly
retrieved autobiographical memories. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 38, 1296–1308.
A. Scoboria et al. / Cognition 136 (2015) 337–349
Wade, K. A., Nash, R., & Garry, M. (2014). People consider reliability and cost
when verifying their autobiographical memories. Acta Psychologica,
146, 28–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.12.001.
Wilson, A. E., & Ross, M. (2003). The identity function of autobiographical
memory: Time is on our side. Memory, 11, 137–149. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1080/741938210.
349
Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A
review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46,
441–517. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2002.2864.