ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
1
ACCEPTED VERSION
Romantic Nostalgia as a Resource for Healthy Relationships
Nicholas D. Evans1, Jacob Juhl2, Erica G. Hepper3,
Tim Wildschut2, Constantine Sedikides2, and Adam K. Fetterman4
1
University of Texas at El Paso
2
University of Southampton
3
4
University of Surrey
University of Houston
Word Count: 10,564
Corresponding author: Nicholas Evans, Psychology Department, University of Texas at El Paso,
500 W University Ave., El Paso, TX, 79902, USA; Email:
[email protected].
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
2
Abstract
Nostalgia is an emotion that confers psychological benefits. The literature has neglected
romantic nostalgia—that is, nostalgia specifically for past experiences shared with one’s
partner—and its potential advantages for relationships. We examined romantic nostalgia in one
correlational study, two experiments, and one daily diary study (N = 638). Romantic nostalgia
was positively associated with greater relationship commitment, satisfaction, and closeness
(Study 1). Additionally, inducing romantic nostalgia via a writing task (Study 2) or music (Study
3) strengthened relational benefits. Finally, participants reported more positive relationshipspecific experiences on days when they felt greater romantic nostalgia (Study 4). We discuss
contributions to the nostalgia and relationships literatures.
Keywords: Romantic nostalgia, closeness, commitment, relationship satisfaction,
romantic relationships
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
3
Romantic Nostalgia as a Resource for Healthy Relationships
Romantic relationships are vital to psychological wellbeing and physical health (Simpson
& Campbell, 2013), and it is therefore crucial to understand how romantic partners can restore,
maintain, and enhance relationship functioning. One potential arrow in their quiver is romantic
nostalgia, defined as nostalgia specifically for past experiences shared with one’s current
romantic partner. Nostalgia, more generally, entails several psychological benefits, including
social connectedness and meaning in life (Sedikides et al., 2015). We built on this theoretical and
empirical foundation to investigate whether romantic nostalgia brings about relationship-specific
benefits.
Romantic Relationships
Humans have a fundamental need to belong that is met, at least in part, through initiating
and maintaining interpersonal bonds, including romantic relationships (Baumeister & Leary,
1995). Romantic relationships frequently become an integral part of one’s self-concept (Aron et
al., 1992, 1995; Mattingly et al., 2020) and confer psychological advantages, including emotional
support (Cramer, 2004), affection (Gulledge et al., 2003), and companionship (Sedikides et al.,
1994). Romantic relationships greatly impact romantic partners and can enhance the quality of
their lives (Braithwaite et al., 2010; Bucher et al., 2019).
The literature has identified several indicators of relationship quality. Closeness (often
referred to as intimacy) is the degree to which partners feel bonded to one another (Dibble et al.,
2012; Sternberg & Grajek, 1984). Satisfaction is the level of positive emotions experienced in
the relationship (i.e., how happy the relationship makes the partners; Rusbult, 1980, 1983).
Finally, commitment is the intent or desire to maintain one’s romantic relationship and work
toward making the relationship last (Rusbult, 1980, 1983). Closeness, satisfaction, and
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
4
commitment are interrelated indicators of relationship quality (Maxwell, 1985; Rusbult et al.,
1998).
The literature has also identified two distinct and positive types of romantic love:
compassionate and passionate. Compassionate love is characterized by respect, trust, affection,
and care when one’s partner is suffering or in need (Sprecher & Fehr, 2005). Passionate love
(also known as the “Eros” love style; Hendrick & Hendrick, 1986) is characterized by a deep
desire or yearning to be with one’s romantic partner (Hatfield & Walster, 1978). Compassionate
and passionate love are highly correlated and are additional indicators of relationship quality.
Both are associated with higher levels of satisfaction and commitment (Acevedo & Aron, 2009;
Fehr et al., 2014; Lemieux & Hale, 1999; Neto, 2021; Neto & Wilks, 2017). Additionally,
compassionate love is linked to increased closeness (Fehr et al., 2014) or intimacy (Neto, 2021),
and passionate love is linked to greater self-disclosure (Kito, 2005), a key component of
closeness (Aron et al., 1997; Sedikides et al., 1999).
To maintain, restore, or enhance relationship functioning, partners deploy various
resources, including trust, empathy and perspective-taking, and gratitude for the partner.
Researchers have extensively documented the beneficial impact of these relationship
maintenance strategies on closeness (Murray & Hazelwood, 2011), satisfaction (Algoe et al.,
2010; Cramer & Jowett, 2010; Davis & Oathout, 1987; Franzoi et al., 1985; Schröder-Abé &
Schütz, 2011), and commitment (Gordon et al., 2012). We posit that nostalgia for past
experiences shared with one’s current romantic partner also improves relationship quality. We
begin with a brief exposition to the nostalgia literature.
Nostalgia
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
5
Nostalgia is a “sentimental longing or wistful affection for the past” (The New Oxford
Dictionary of English, 1998, p. 1266). Prototype analyses have illustrated that the central
features of nostalgia include reminiscence, longing, fond and rose-colored memories, happiness
and enjoyment, as well as desire to re-live to the past. Narrative analyses have revealed that,
when nostalgic, people commonly recall meaningful life events (family re-unions, graduations,
weddings), time periods (e.g., childhood, college), and places (e.g., grandma’s house, vacation
resort); moreover, in nostalgic narratives, people nearly always place themselves among close
others (family, friends; Abeyta et al., 2015; Wildschut, Sedikides, Arndt, & Routledge, 2006).
Further, multidimensional scaling analyses have revealed that nostalgia is a positively valenced,
approach-oriented, and low arousal emotion (Van Tilburg et al., 2018). Although nostalgia is
largely positive, nostalgic reverie is frequently bittersweet: It can contain traces of sadness, as
people desire to re-experience the past (Madoglou et al., 2017; Sedikides & Wildschut, 2016).
Nostalgia bears several psychological benefits. For example, it strengthens a sense of
meaning in life (Sedikides & Wildschut, 2018), optimism (Cheung et al., 2013), self-continuity
(Sedikides et al., 2016), positive self-regard (Vess et al., 2012), and social connectedness
(Frankenbach et al., 2021). Additionally, nostalgia is a psychological resource to which people
turn when they face psychological adversity (Routledge et al., 2013; Wildschut & Sedikides,
2022). For example, meaninglessness (Routledge et al., 2011), death thoughts (Juhl et al., 2010),
and loneliness (Zhou et al., 2008) increase nostalgia.
Romantic Nostalgia
As mentioned, people are nostalgic for many aspects from their past (e.g., events, time
periods, places). One of the most frequent targets of nostalgic reflection is others: friends, family,
and romantic partners (Abeyta et al., 2015; Madoglou et al., 2017). In this article, we put a
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
6
microscope on nostalgia, focusing on past experiences that are shared with one’s current
romantic partner—that is, romantic nostalgia. Just as nostalgia in general (henceforth, referred to
as general nostalgia),1 bears psychological benefits (e.g., meaning in life, social connectedness),
so does romantic nostalgia have relationship-specific benefits.
The rationale for this proposal is as follows. First, general nostalgia, which largely
contains thoughts about others, increases perceptions of connectedness with others (Frankenbach
et al., 2021). It stands to reason that romantic nostalgia, which entails directly reflecting on one’s
romantic partner, should similarly increase perceptions of closeness with one’s romantic partner.
Second, general nostalgia elevates perceived meaning in life (Routledge et al., 2011). In the same
vein, romantic nostalgia should elevate the perceived meaningfulness and value that partners
place on their romantic relationship. Third, when nostalgic, people primarily ponder positive
events often through rose-colored glasses (Wildschut et al, 2006). They may also ponder
ambivalent events, but the relevant narratives typically follow a redemption sequence (McAdams
et al., 2001) whereby initial adversity gives way to increased positivity and a happy ending (e.g.,
life lessons learned; Wildschut et al., 2006). Thus, when experiencing romantic nostalgia, it is
likely that partners focus on positive relationship experiences or frame relationship experiences
in a redemptive trajectory. This should, in turn, increase relationship closeness, satisfaction, and
commitment. Finally, nostalgizing about one’s romantic relationship may heighten the
accessibility of one’s investment into their relationship. According to the investment model of
relationships (Rusbult, 1980), greater relationship investment strengthens relationship
commitment.
1
We do not advocate the use of the term general nostalgia outside the context of this article. We only use
it here for clarity.
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
7
The literature provides indirect evidence for our proposal that romantic nostalgia
improves perception of relationship quality. For example, people who enjoy telling the story of
their romantic relationships and view the course of their relationships as a coherent narrative
report higher relationship satisfaction (Dunlop, 2019). Indeed, those who mention emotionally
positive experiences when telling the story of their romantic relationship, evince greater
relationship satisfaction (Dunlop, 2020). Also, those who discuss intimate topics with their
partner while telling stories of relationship events report greater relationship satisfaction and
closeness, and fewer thoughts of breaking up (Frost, 2013). In somewhat more direct evidence
for our proposal, when recalling relationship-defining memories, people experience an enhanced
sense of intimacy, commitment, and relationship satisfaction (Alea & Bluck, 2007; Alea & Vick,
2010; Guilbault & Philippe, 2017).
Yet, direct evidence on romantic nostalgia is scarce. Muise et al. (2020) investigated
sexual nostalgia. On the surface, sexual nostalgia might be considered a subtype of romantic
nostalgia, as sexual experiences frequently occur with romantic partners. However, the authors
defined sexual nostalgia as “reflection on positive sexual experiences with past partners” (italics
added, p. 1538), and found that it occurs among people with unmet sexual needs in their current
relationships. Perhaps most directly relevant to our proposal, Mallory et al. (2018) developed a
self-report measure of romantic nostalgia and examined its association with indices of
relationship functioning in two studies. Their findings were mixed and difficult to interpret.
Cross-sectional Study 1 tested college students who were in an exclusive dating relationship.
Romantic nostalgia was positively associated with relationship satisfaction but negatively
associated with benign attributions about one’s partner. Longitudinal Study 2 tested college
students involved in committed relationships. They completed measures of romantic nostalgia
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
8
and relationship satisfaction at three time points. In cross-lagged analyses, Time 1 relationship
satisfaction predicted decreased romantic nostalgia at Time 2, and Time 1 romantic nostalgia
predicted decreased relationship satisfaction at Time 2. Inconsistent with our proposal, these
findings suggest a negative reciprocal relationship between romantic nostalgia and relationship
satisfaction. Yet, analysis of Time 3 data failed to replicate these results. Interpretation of these
findings is further clouded by the assessment of romantic nostalgia. Participants rated the degree
to which they missed various aspects of the early parts of their relationship with their current
partner (e.g., “Times when this person made you laugh,” “Times you spent talking with this
person”; 1 = do not miss at all, 5 = miss very much/very frequently). Endorsement of these items
implies a sense of loss or relationship deterioration, and, although “missing” is a prototypical
feature of nostalgia, it is not a highly central one (Hepper et al., 2012, 2014). Therefore, the
focus of these items is on the relatively narrow negative scope of nostalgia, rather than the more
prominent positive experience of the emotion (Hepper et al., 2021; Layous et al., 2021). In
addition, the correlational nature of both studies limits causal inferences. In summary, there is
solid ground to expect that romantic nostalgia improves perceptions of relationship quality. We
turn to an examination of this proposal.
Overview
In four studies, we examined correlates and benefits of romantic nostalgia, capitalizing on
validated measures and experimental manipulations from the nostalgia literature. Our
investigation focused on perceptions of relationship quality—an important facet of actual
relationship quality (Maxwell, 1985; Rusbult et al., 1998). In Study 1, we tested associations
between self-reported romantic nostalgia and indicators of relationship quality (i.e., closeness,
satisfaction, commitment). In Studies 2 and 3, we assessed the causal effect of romantic
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
9
nostalgia—induced via writing and music, respectively—on perceived relationship quality.
Lastly, in Study 4, we examined romantic nostalgia in daily life. We offer specific hypotheses in
each study’s introduction. We provided complete stimulus materials in Supplementary Material
and deposited all data at https://tinyurl.com/e4x8p4zy.2
Study 1
Study 1 served as an empirical foray into the correlates of romantic nostalgia. We
measured romantic nostalgia as well as perceived relationship closeness, commitment, and
satisfaction—three of the six components of relationship quality (Fletcher et al., 2000). We
hypothesized that self-reported romantic nostalgia would be positively associated with
relationship closeness, satisfaction, and commitment.3
Method
Participants
We recruited 251 MTurk workers from the United States, who participated in exchange
for $0.50. Participants had to be at least 18 years old and currently in a romantic relationship. To
determine sample size, we relied on Schönbrodt and Perugini (2013), who found that correlations
between r = .1 and r = .2 stabilize between 238 and 252 participants. Therefore, we aimed for
250.
2
We preregistered Studies 1 and 3 on the Open Science Framework. The preregistration for Study 1 can
be accessed at https://tinyurl.com/wzy4xazb. The preregistration for Study 3 can be accessed at
https://tinyurl.com/tmwr29jf.
3
We deviated from the preregistered hypotheses and analysis plan. After consultation with the research
team, we omitted one variable, conflict compromise, because its relevance to the current investigation
was unclear. Originally, we hypothesized that romantic nostalgia would be associated with closeness,
satisfaction, and commitment, above and beyond conflict compromise. Therefore, as we omitted conflict
compromise, we simply assessed the bivariate associations between romantic nostalgia and the three
putative relational benefits. Nevertheless, when controlling for conflict compromise, romantic nostalgia
significantly correlated with closeness, r(244) = .22, p < .001, and relationship satisfaction, r(244) = .19,
p = .002, but not commitment, r(244) = .11, p = .102.
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
10
We removed from analyses four participants who were not currently in a romantic
relationship and two participants for inattentive responding. The final sample comprised 245
participants (127 cisgender men, 116 cisgender women, 2 nonbinary; age: M = 32.38 years, SD =
9.10 years; relationship duration: M = 82.93 months, SD = 92.34 months; 75.5% White, 11.0%
Hispanic/Latinx, 10.2% Asian American/Pacific Islander, 6.9% Black/African American, 3.0%
Native American, 0.4% Arab American4). A sensitivity analysis (G*Power 3.1; Faul et al., 2013)
indicated that our sample size allowed us to detect small-to-medium effects (r = .13) with
acceptable power of (1 – β) = .80 at α = .05 (two-tailed).
Materials and Procedures
Participants completed the key measures of romantic nostalgia, closeness, satisfaction,
and commitment in random order.
Romantic Nostalgia. To measure romantic nostalgia, we adapted items from the 7-item
Southampton Nostalgia Scale (Sedikides et al., 2015). Although the original Southampton
Nostalgia Scale assesses the importance of nostalgia to participants as well as the frequency with
which they experience nostalgia, it was recently shown to be unidimensional (Biskas et al.,
2021). Even so, we additionally tested and confirmed the satisfactory model fit of the singlefactor structure model for the adapted romantic nostalgia measure (see Supplementary Material
for model fit indices and factor loadings). Participants read a definition of nostalgia (“a
sentimental longing for the past”) and responded to the seven items, modified such that each
referred to romantic nostalgia. The items assessed the importance that participants placed on
romantic nostalgia (e.g., “How valuable is romantic nostalgia to you?”) and the frequency with
which they experienced romantic nostalgic (e.g., “How often do you experience nostalgia about
4
For all studies, participants were given the option to select any and all ethnicity/race options with which
they identify. Therefore, the sum of the percentages for race/ethnicity categories exceeds 100%.
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
11
your romantic partner and/or romantic relationship?”; 1 = not at all, 7 = very much). Thus,
departing from Mallory et al. (2018), our measure did not focus exclusively on the early parts of
participants’ relationships and did not ask participants to rate how much they “miss” those
experiences. We averaged responses to compute romantic nostalgia scores (M = 4.40, SD = 1.47,
α = .93).
Relationship Closeness. To measure closeness, we used the 11-item Unidimensional
Relationship Closeness Scale (Dibble et al., 2012; e.g., “My relationship with my romantic
partner is close;” 1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). We averaged responses to calculate
relationship closeness scores (M = 5.91, SD = 1.02, α = .94).
Relationship Satisfaction. We used the Satisfaction subscale of the Investment Model
Scale (Rusbult et al., 1998) to measure relationship satisfaction. Participants indicated the extent
to which they agreed with five items (e.g., “I feel satisfied with our relationship;” 1 = strongly
disagree, 7 = strongly agree). We averaged responses to compute relationship satisfaction scores
(M = 5.60, SD = 1.20, α = .90).
Relationship Commitment. We assessed relationship commitment with the
Commitment facet of the Investment Model Scale (Rusbult et al., 1998). Participants indicated
the extent to which they agreed with seven items (e.g., “I want our relationship to last for a very
long time;” 1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). We averaged responses to compute
relationship commitment scores (M = 5.85, SD = 1.19, α = .86).
Results and Discussion
We tested the associations between romantic nostalgia and the putative relational benefits
(Table 1). As hypothesized, romantic nostalgia positively correlated with closeness, satisfaction,
and commitment. Prior work demonstrated a link between relationship length and commitment
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
12
(Rusbult et al., 1998). Relationship length may therefore confound the association between
romantic nostalgia and the benefits. Indeed, relationship length positively correlated with
commitment. However, it did not significantly correlate with romantic nostalgia. Nevertheless,
we computed partial correlations between romantic nostalgia and benefits, controlling for
relationship length. These partial correlations showed that romantic nostalgia positively
correlated with closeness, r(244) = .24, p < .001, satisfaction, r(244) = .21, p < .001, and
commitment, r(244) = .17, p = .010.
Study 1 provided initial support for an association between romantic nostalgia and
benefits: Partners who were more romantically nostalgic reported greater closeness, satisfaction,
and commitment. However, given that we measured rather than manipulated romantic nostalgia,
we could not make claims about its causal influence on relational benefits. We addressed this
limitation in Study 2.
Study 2
To test the causal effects of romantic nostalgia, we experimentally induced it and
measured ensuing relational benefits. We hypothesized that romantic nostalgia (vs. control)
would bolster perceived relationship closeness, satisfaction, and commitment.
Method
Participants
We determined the sample size for Study 2 by conducting a power analysis (G*Power;
Faul et al., 2013). Previous research has indicated that nostalgia manipulations produce medium
effect sizes (Evans et al., 2021; Juhl et al., 2021; Sedikides et al., 2015). Our power analysis
revealed that the minimum sample size to detect an effect size of f = .25 at power = .80 and α =
.05 (two-tailed) was 128. We initially recruited 186 students from a large Southwestern
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
13
university in the United States (for course credit), anticipating attrition. We removed the data of
18 participants who were not involved in a romantic relationship at the time of the study and 34
participants who did not complete the manipulation task (e.g., wrote nothing) or the dependent
measures. The final sample consisted of 134 participants (98 women, 36 men; M = 20.31 years,
SD = 3.22 years; 91.8% Hispanic/Latinx, 11.9% White, 2.2% Black/African American, 2.2%
Asian American/Pacific Islander, 0.7% Middle Eastern, 0.7% unspecified Multi-ethnic/Multiracial).5
Procedures
We administered the experiment on Qualtrics, which participants were able to complete
on their own time. Participants first filled out a demographics questionnaire. Next, they were
randomly assigned to the romantic nostalgia (n = 57) or control (n = 77) condition. Subsequently,
they responded to a manipulation check and the relationship closeness, commitment, and
satisfaction scales.6
Materials
Romantic Nostalgia Induction. The Event Reflection Task, for which participants
reflect on and write about either a nostalgic or ordinary autobiographical memory, is the most
commonly used technique to induce nostalgia (Leunissen et al., 2021; Sedikides et al., 2015). We
adapted this technique to elicit romantic nostalgia. In the romantic nostalgia condition, we first
provided the formal definition of nostalgia (The New Oxford Dictionary of English, 1998). We
then instructed participants to reflect on a nostalgic experience with their current romantic
partner, and to write about it for three minutes. We instructed those in the control condition to
5
Due to experimenter error, we did not assess relationship duration in Study 2.
Participants also completed a measure assessing social connectedness. The measure does not specifically
pertain to romantic relationships. We present relevant results in Supplementary Material.
6
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
14
reflect on an ordinary experience with their current romantic partner, and to write about it for
three minutes. Subsequently, participants automatically progressed to the manipulation check.
Manipulation Check. Participants responded to two statements (i.e., “Right now, I am
feeling quite nostalgic,” “Right now, I’m having nostalgic feelings”) gauging their levels of felt
nostalgia (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree). We averaged responses to produce
manipulation check scores (M = 3.96, SD = 1.09, r = .86).
Closeness. Participants then completed the Unidimensional Relationship Closeness Scale
described in Study 1 (Dibble et al., 2012; M = 6.26, SD = 0.75, α = .92).
Satisfaction and Commitment. Finally, participants filled out the Satisfaction (M =
5.95, SD = 0.99, α = .87) and Commitment (M = 5.97, SD = 0.97, α = .81) facets of the
Investment Model Scale described in Study 1 (Rusbult et al., 1998).
Results and Discussion
Preliminary Analyses
We expected the dependent variables (i.e., closeness, satisfaction, commitment) to be
intercorrelated. Indeed, this was the case. Closeness correlated significantly with satisfaction,
r(129) = .70, p < .001, and commitment r(128) = .77, p < .001; satisfaction correlated
significantly with commitment, r(131) = .63, p < .001. To test the effectiveness of the
manipulation, we conducted a one-way (romantic nostalgia vs. control) Analysis of Variance
(ANOVA) on the manipulation check. As intended, participants in the romantic nostalgia
condition (M = 4.32, SD = 0.82) felt more nostalgic than those in the control condition (M =
3.69, SD = 1.18), F(1, 132) = 11.84, p < .001, η² = .082, 90% CI [.023, .165].7
Effects of Romantic Nostalgia
7
We report 90% confidence intervals for η², because the F-test is one-sided (Steiger, 2004).
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
15
To test the effects of romantic nostalgia on closeness, satisfaction, and commitment, we
conducted a one-way (romantic nostalgia vs. control) ANOVA on each. Participants in the
romantic nostalgia condition felt somewhat closer (although this difference was not statistically
significant), reported greater relationship satisfaction, and felt more committed to their partners
than those in the control condition (Table 2). In all, Study 2 demonstrated that romantic nostalgia
confers relational benefits.
Study 3
In Study 3, we sought to replicate and extend Study 2 findings by using a different
technique to manipulate romantic nostalgia. Research has established that music is a potent
nostalgia trigger (Barrett et al., 2010; Cheung et al., 2013; Stephan et al., 2015). Thus, in the
present study, we induced romantic nostalgia through songs that hold nostalgic significance to
participants’ romantic relationships.
As in Study 2, we measured closeness, satisfaction, and commitment after the romantic
nostalgia induction. However, we also intended to examine whether romantic nostalgia confers
other putative benefits. In particular, we tested whether romantic nostalgia augments
connectedness to one’s romantic partner (i.e., romantic connectedness) and boosts optimism
regarding the future of one’s relationship (i.e., relationship optimism). We considered romantic
connectedness an additional measure of relationship closeness. Relationship optimism is a
component of relationship commitment; it reflects a long-term orientation toward one’s
relationship and the sense that a relationship will be sustained over time (Arriaga & Agnew,
2001; Rusbult & Buunk, 1993).
Additionally, we assessed the two distinct types of romantic love: compassionate
(Sprecher & Fehr, 2005) and passionate (Hatfield & Walster, 1978). Although these types of
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
16
love are often measured as more stable characteristics, both can be assessed as states that ebb and
flow based on external influences (Hatfield & Sprecher, 1986; Sprecher & Fehr, 2005). They
map respectively onto the love and passion components of relationship quality, meaning that
Study 3 covered five of the six components (Fletcher et al., 2000). We hypothesized that
participants who listened to a romantically nostalgic song (vs. control: non-nostalgic song)
would experience stronger closeness, satisfaction, commitment, romantic connectedness,
relationship optimism, compassionate love, and passionate love.
Method
Participants
We determined the minimal sample size (N = 172) based on the same effect size as Study
2 (f = .25) at power = .90 and α = .05 (two-tailed). However, due to COVID-19, we could only
recruit 151 students (earning course credit) from a large Southwestern university in the United
States before all studies were shut down. All participants were currently in romantic relationships
(124 cisgender women, 27 cisgender men; age: M = 21.43 years, SD = 4.30 years; relationship
duration: M = 29.83 months, SD = 27.75 months; 93.4% Hispanic/Latinx, 25.8% White, 1.3%
Black/African American, 0.6% Native American, 0.6% Middle Eastern). Due to our lower than
desired sample size, we conducted a sensitivity analysis (G*Power 3.1; Faul et al., 2013), which
indicated that our sample size allowed us to detect small-to-medium effects (f = .23) with
acceptable power of (1 – β) = .80 at α = .05 (two-tailed), matching Study 2’s power.
Procedure and Materials
Romantic Nostalgia Music Induction. We randomly assigned participants either to a
romantic nostalgia (n = 75) or control (n = 76) condition. In the romantic nostalgia condition, we
first provided the formal definition of nostalgia. Then, we asked participants to think of a song
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
17
that reminded them of their romantic relationship and made them feel nostalgic. In the control
condition, we asked participants to think of a song they enjoyed and was unrelated to their
romantic relationship. In both conditions, we instructed participants to write the name of the song
and the artist who performs it on a sheet of paper next to the computer, and notify the
experimenter. The experimenter opened Spotify® and cued up each participant’s song.
Participants then listened to the entire song on Sennheiser headphones.
Manipulation Check. Participants completed the same two-item manipulation check as
in Study 3, indicating their response using a slider (0 = strongly disagree, 100 = strongly agree;
M = 61.67, SD = 33.09, r = .91).
Closeness. Participants completed the Unidimensional Relationship Closeness Scale as in
Studies 1 and 2 (Dibble et al., 2012) using a slider (0 = strongly disagree, 100 = strongly agree).
We averaged responses to create an index of relationship closeness (M = 88.04, SD = 12.38, α =
.89).
Satisfaction and Commitment. Participants filled out the Investment Model Scale as in
Studies 1 and 2 (Rusbult et al., 1998), using a slider (0 = strongly disagree, 100 = strongly
agree), to assess satisfaction (M = 84.76, SD = 15.27, α = .85) and commitment (M = 88.93, SD
= 14.82, α = .85).
Romantic Connectedness and Relationship Optimism. We adapted prior measures of
social connectedness (Wildschut et al., 2006) and optimism (Cheung et al., 2013) to assess
romantic connectedness (e.g., “With this song in mind, I feel connected to my romantic partner”)
and relationship optimism (e.g., “With this song in mind, I feel optimistic about my future with
my romantic partner”). For each measure, participants indicated their agreement with four
statements using a slider (0 = strongly disagree, 100 = strongly agree). We averaged responses
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
18
to compute romantic connectedness (M = 60.98, SD = 38.16, α = .97) and relationship optimism
(M = 65.36, SD = 35.04, α = .96) scores.
Compassionate Love. Participants filled out the Compassionate Love Scale (Sprecher &
Fehr, 2005). They indicated their agreement with 21 statements (e.g., “I feel considerable
compassionate love for my romantic partner”) using a slider (0 = strongly disagree, 100 =
strongly agree). We averaged responses to compute compassionate love scores (M = 87.66, SD =
10.58, α = .90).
Passionate Love. Participants completed the shortened version of the Passionate Love
Scale (Hatfield & Sprecher, 1986). They indicated their agreement with 15 statements (e.g., “For
me, my romantic partner is perfect”) using a slider (0 = not at all true, 100 = definitely true). We
averaged across responses to compute passionate love scores (M = 76.30, SD = 15.60, α = .88).
Results and Discussion
Preliminary Analyses
All variables were intercorrelated, with the exception of romantic connectedness and
commitment (Table 3). To test the effectiveness of the manipulation, we conducted a one-way
(romantic nostalgia vs. control) ANOVA on the manipulation check. As intended, participants in
the romantic nostalgia condition (M = 81.51, SD = 19.38) felt more nostalgic than those in the
control condition (M = 41.84, SD = 32.11), F(1, 148) = 83.87, p < .001, η² = .362, 90% CI [.264,
.450].
Effects of Romantic Nostalgia
To test the influence of romantic nostalgia on relational benefits, we conducted a series of
one-way ANOVAs (romantic nostalgia vs. control; Table 4). As hypothesized, participants in the
romantic nostalgia (vs. control) condition reported higher levels of romantic connectedness,
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
19
relationship optimism, closeness, satisfaction, compassionate love, and passionate love. Those in
the romantic nostalgia condition also reported higher commitment than those in the control
condition, although this difference was not statistically significant. Taken together, the Study 3
findings replicated and extended those of Study 2 using a different romantic nostalgia induction,
and so adding support to the hypothesis that romantic nostalgia strengthens perceptions of
relationship quality. 8
Study 4
In Study 1, we showed that romantic nostalgia is prognostic of better relationship quality,
and, in Studies 2-3, we provided evidence for the causal role of romantic nostalgia in producing
these relational benefits. In Study 4, we used a daily diary design to examine the link between
romantic nostalgia and relationship functioning in daily life. As in Study 3, we assessed romantic
connectedness and relationship optimism. Unique to this study, we were also concerned with a
behavioral intention indicative of relationship (dis)satisfaction. Specifically, we assessed
participants’ desire to leave their romantic partner. We also assessed, as a control variable, the
amount of time participants spent time with their romantic partners. We hypothesized that, when
controlling for time spent with one’s romantic partner, daily romantic nostalgia would be
positively associated with romantic connectedness and relationship optimism, and negatively
associated with the desire to leave one’s romantic partner.9
Method
Participants
8
Given that listening to music can evoke positive affect (Koelsch, 2013), we re-analyzed our data
controlling for overall positive affect. All effects remained significant.
9
Study 4 involved a time-intensive protocol; for efficiency, we included several items to test hypotheses
unrelated to the current investigation as well as participants uninvolved in romantic relationships. Results
pertinent to these other items were reported in a recently published article (Evans et al., 2021), which does
not mention any of the variables measured and tested in Study 4.
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
20
We followed the sampling plan of prior investigations that have used similar daily diary
designs (Fetterman et al., 2018; Lenton et al., 2016); that is, we aimed to collect data from
approximately 100 participants, a typical sample size used in daily diary research (Ohly et al.,
2010). Accordingly, we collected data from 108 students from a large Southwestern university in
the United States (86 cisgender women, 22 cisgender men; age: M = 20.46 years, SD = 3.65
years; 90.7% Hispanic/Latinx, 26.9% White, 0.9% Black/African American, 1.9% Asian
American/Pacific Islander, 0.9% Native American), all of whom were currently in a romantic
relationship (duration: M = 29.58 months, SD = 33.87 months). All participants, who earned
extra course credit, completed at least one daily survey (out of a possible 14). On average, they
completed 9.89 daily surveys, producing 1068 observations.
Materials and Procedures
We advertised the study for one week. Upon signing up, participants filled out an initial
assessment, which comprised several individual difference measures and a demographic
questionnaire.10 Then, they completed the same daily survey for 14 days, starting the Monday
after the recruitment week. Participants received an email at 5:00 p.m. on each of these 14 days.
This email contained a link to that day’s survey, which was available until 3:00 a.m. the next
morning. We randomized the daily items every day for each participant.
We measured each daily construct with a single, face-valid item. For romantic nostalgia,
participants responded to the item: “Today, I thought back to a special time with my romantic
partner” (1 = strongly disagree, 4 = strongly agree; M = 2.80, SD = 1.09). For romantic
10
Within the initial assessment, participants also completed the romantic relationship adaptation of the
Southampton Nostalgia Scale (Sedikides et al., 2015) used in Study 1 as an individual difference measure
of romantic nostalgia (M = 4.90, SD = 1.35, α = .93). An exploratory analysis revealed that trait romantic
nostalgia positively predicted daily romantic nostalgia, b = .096, t(105) = 2.04, p = .043, 95% CI [.002,
.190].
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
21
connectedness, participants rated the item: “Today, I felt connected to my romantic partner” (1 =
not at all, 4 = very much; M = 2.93, SD = 1.09). We assessed relationship optimism with the
item: “Today, I felt optimistic about my romantic relationship” (1 = not at all, 4 = very much; M
= 3.02, SD = 1.05). Desire to leave their romantic partner was captured by the item: “Today, I
considered leaving my romantic partner” (1 = strongly disagree, 4 = strongly agree; M = 1.53,
SD = 0.95). Finally, to index time spent with their partner, participants responded to the item:
“Today, I spent time with my romantic partner” (1 = not at all, 4 = very much; M = 2.48, SD =
1.22).
Results
To test our hypotheses concerning the within-person (i.e., daily) associations, we
employed a multi-level modeling (MLM) approach (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). This approach
allowed us to distinguish within-person from between-person sources of variance (Enders &
Tofighi, 2007). We used the “lme” function within the “nlme” package (Pinheiro et al., 2020) in
R. In addition to our fixed effects, we included corresponding random effects to specify standard
errors regarding our Level 1 (within-subject) predictors (Barr et al., 2013). To account for
individual differences in daily experiences of romantic nostalgia and amount of time spent with
romantic partners, we centered these two variables around each person’s mean scores (Enders &
Tofighi, 2007). Moreover, the analyses included a first-order autoregressive error structure to
account for similarities between adjacent days. We tested the unique link between daily romantic
nostalgia and, respectively, daily romantic connectedness, daily relationship optimism, and daily
desire to leave, with and without controlling for time spent with one’s romantic partner each day.
Therefore, we tested two models for each daily relationship outcome:
Model 1:
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
22
yij = β0j + β1j (Romantic Nostalgia) + rij
β0j = γ00 + u0j
β1j = γ10 + u1j
Model 2:
yij = β0j + β1j (Time Spent with Partner) + β2j (Romantic Nostalgia) + rij
β0j = γ00 + u0j
β1j = γ10 + u1j
β2j = γ20 + u2j
We present the results of these analyses in Table 5. Daily romantic nostalgia uniquely
predicted each daily relationship benefit. More precisely, on days that participants felt more
romantically nostalgic, they also felt more connected to their romantic partner, more optimistic
about their relationship, and less inclined to leave their romantic partner—above and beyond the
amount of time they spent with their romantic partners. The Study 4 findings demonstrate, with
high ecological validity, that romantic nostalgia is linked to relational benefits in daily life.
General Discussion
We examined, in four studies, whether romantic nostalgia conveys relational benefits. In
Study 1, we showed that romantic nostalgia is positively associated with perceived closeness,
satisfaction, and commitment. In the next two studies, we experimentally induced romantic
nostalgia, via written narratives (Study 2) and music (Study 3), to test its causal effect on
relational benefits. In Study 2, romantic nostalgia strengthened closeness, satisfaction, and
commitment. In Study 3, romantic nostalgia had similar effects on closeness and satisfaction (but
not on commitment), while also augmenting perceptions of romantic connectedness, relationship
optimism, compassionate love, and passionate love. These effects remained significant after
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
23
controlling for positive affect, demonstrating that the benefits of romantic nostalgia are more
than simple positivity. In Study 4, we conceptually replicated these findings in an ecologically
valid context. On days when participants reported feeling more nostalgic for their relationship,
they also reported stronger connectedness to their partner, greater optimism about the future of
their relationship, and a weaker intention to terminate their relationship. These associations
remained significant when controlling for the time participants spent with their romantic partner
throughout the day.
Implications
Our research fits within and expands on the nostalgia literature. Prior work has shown
that general nostalgia fosters social bonds (Frankenbach et al., 2021; Sedikides & Wildschut,
2019). However, the relevance of romantic nostalgia for specific close relationships has been
relatively unexplored. The results highlight romantic nostalgia’s positive role in close
relationships. Our findings, therefore, point to new empirical directions regarding relationshipspecific benefits of romantic nostalgia. For example, given that romantic nostalgia promoted
compassionate love (Study 3), it might increase empathy (cf. Juhl et al., 2020) and motivate
helping of one’s romantic partner (Sprecher & Fehr, 2005). Romantic nostalgia may also serve as
a resource to buffer relationship threats: it might help partners maintain intimacy and
commitment when they are separated for long periods, or help partners respond to romantic
conflict in a constructive rather than destructive manner (cf. Abeyta et al., 2015). Finally,
partners might turn to romantic nostalgia when feeling dissatisfied. Muise et al. (2020) reported
that individuals nostalgize about sexual experiences in past relationships when their sexual needs
in the current relationship are unmet. Although such sexual nostalgia does not seem to enhance
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
24
(and may even hinder) sexual or relationship satisfaction, nostalgia for sex with one’s current
romantic partner may revitalize one’s sexual life and enhance relational benefits.
Our findings have applied implications. Most notably, romantic nostalgia could be
harnessed in a therapeutic setting to manage relationship woes. For example, when the bond
between romantic partners cracks and relationship dissatisfaction sets in, one’s well-being will
inevitably suffer. Rather than ruminating on relationship dissatisfaction (Jostmann et al., 2011),
our work suggests that nostalgizing about the relationship entails the potential to restore
perceptions of relationship quality. Indeed, individuals experiencing relationship dysfunction
might derive the greatest benefit from romantic nostalgia. The current studies provide empirical
support for the utility of romantic nostalgia as a resource for healthy relationship functioning.
Assuming replication and extension of our findings, relationship and marital counselors might be
encouraged to incorporate romantic nostalgia as a tool for re-instating relationship functioning.
By building on the ensuing literature, nostalgia and relationship researchers may foster a new
means of approaching relationship conflict in a therapeutic setting.
Limitations and Future Directions
The studies in which we experimentally induced romantic nostalgia through writing
(Study 2) and music (Study 3) provide causal evidence for the effect of romantic nostalgia on
perceptions of relationship quality. However, our findings do not rule out the possibility that the
direction of causality could also run in the opposite direction. That is, greater relationship quality
could cause people to be more nostalgia about their relationship. In turn, romantic nostalgia, as
we have shown, should then further increase perceptions of relationship quality, resulting in a
cyclical pattern. Future work should investigate this possibility.
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
25
Prior research has shown that thoughts about negative experiences in one’s relationship
are associated with unfavorable perceptions of one’s relationship (Fincham & Linfield, 1997;
Fincham & Rogge, 2010; Mattson et al., 2013). In light of the present research, however,
reflecting on negative relationship experiences nostalgically may not produce such unfavorable
consequences. As previously mentioned, nostalgic memories are largely, but not wholly,
positive. Some nostalgic narratives contain negative past experiences. However, when they do
contain negative experiences, they typically follow a redemptive sequence wherein an individual
reflects on overcoming adversity or personal growth (Wildschut et al., 2006). For example, if a
person nostalgically recalls a time when they had a fight with their partner, they would likely
reframe it as surviving through adversity. Future research should examine this possibility.
Our studies focused on one partner’s perceptions of relationship quality, and more
specifically relational closeness, satisfaction, commitment, and love. Although such perceptions
are key components of relationship quality, and arguably the most valid indicator for that
individual (Fletcher et al., 2000), they form only part of the story of relationship functioning.
Future research ought to examine additional processes such as support, conflict, accommodation,
and forgiveness, and additional measures such as partner perceptions, coded couple behaviors,
and breakup rates in order to ascertain the reach of romantic nostalgia.
We mentioned the therapeutic potential for romantic nostalgia to address and tackle
relationship woes. However, to lay the groundwork for this, future research needs to take several
steps. First, participants in our studies were generally satisfied with their relationships (i.e., they
consistently scored above the midpoint on our outcome measures). It is thus possible that the
benefits of romantic nostalgia only ensue when relationship satisfaction is relatively high. Future
research should seek to replicate our findings among those with below average relationship
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
26
satisfaction. Second, we assessed positive indicators of relationship quality. However, in a
therapeutic context, romantic partners are likely to engage in reparative behaviors such as
sacrifice, accommodation, and forgiveness. Assessing these outcomes in the future would
provide a more comprehensive understanding of romantic nostalgia’s utility as a therapeutic
resource. Third, our studies examined the short-term benefits of romantic nostalgia. That is, we
briefly induced romantic nostalgia and then immediately measured relationship outcomes. If
romantic nostalgia is to be implemented in therapeutic settings, researchers need to develop and
test romantic nostalgia interventions that produce long-term benefits. Fourth, we only induced
romantic nostalgia in one partner. However, therapeutic interventions will often involve both
partners. Prior work investigating collaborative remembering between romantic partners
provides some preliminary evidence that points toward the potential for co-nostalgizing to serve
relationship benefits. For example, romantic dyads co-reminiscing on shared laughter
experiences report higher levels of relationship satisfaction relative to control romantic dyads
(Bazzini et al., 2007). Therefore, future work could building on these findings to examine how
partners’ co-nostalgizing in conversation (cf. Dodd et al., 2021) influences their relationship.
Additionally, a promising direction for future research is to test how attachment
orientations shape the effects of romantic nostalgia. Attachment avoidance moderates how
general nostalgia serves its social-oriented function. That is, participants low in avoidant
attachment are particularly likely to rely on nostalgia to garner social connectedness when
feeling lonely (Abakoumkin et al., 2017; Wildschut et al., 2010). More relevant to the current
article, Juhl et al. (2012) found that, when experiencing general nostalgia, those low (vs. high) in
attachment avoidance reported higher relationship satisfaction. Individuals low in attachment
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
27
avoidance, then, may be most likely to leverage romantic nostalgia in order to reap its relational
benefits.
Finally, for three of our four studies (Studies 2-4), we recruited college students. Future
research should seek to use samples that are more representative of the general population.
Relatedly, in these three studies, participants had been in their relationships for a relatively short
time. Attaining samples more representative of the general population will enable researchers to
capture the effects of romantic nostalgia in longer-term relationships. Similarly, we did not
assess several participant characteristic variables, such as class, socioeconomic status, or
disability information. Although we had no theoretical rationale as to how these variables might
affect our findings, future research on romantic nostalgia would nevertheless benefit from
measuring these demographics.
Conclusion
In four methodologically diverse studies, we took steps to lift the shroud of uncertainty
that has so far obscured the role of romantic nostalgia in relational context. Our findings
documented the multifaceted benefits of romantic nostalgia for relationship functioning in both
laboratory and naturalistic settings. We hope that this work inspires efforts to understand and
harness this powerful resource for restoring, maintaining, and enhancing romantic relationships.
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
28
References
Abakoumkin, G., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., & Bakarou, M. (2017). Nostalgia in response to
group-based exclusion: The role of attachment-related avoidance. European Journal of
Social Psychology, 47(3), 373-381. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2235
Abeyta, A. A., Routledge, C., & Juhl, J. (2015). Looking back to move forward: Nostalgia as a
psychological resource for promoting relationship goals and overcoming relationship
challenges. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(6), 1029-1044.
https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000036
Acevedo, B. P., & Aron, A. (2009). Does a long-term relationship kill romantic love? Review of
General Psychology, 13(1), 59-65. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014226
Alea, N., & Bluck, S. (2007). I’ll keep you in mind: The intimacy function of autobiographical
memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21(1), 1091-1111.
https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1316
Alea, N., Singer, J. A., & Labunko, B. (2015). “We-ness” in relationship-defining memories and
marital satisfaction. In K. Skerrett & K. Fergus (Eds.), Couple resilience (162-177).
Springer.
Alea, N., & Vick, S. C. (2010). The first sight of love: Relationship-defining memories and
marital satisfaction across adulthood. Memory, 18(7), 730-742.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2010.506443
Algoe, S. B., Gable, S. L., & Maisel, N. C. (2010). It’s the little things: Everyday gratitude as a
booster shot for romantic relationships. Personal Relationships, 17(2010), 217-233.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2010.01273.x
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
29
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure
of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(4), 596-612.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.596
Aron, A., Melinat, D., Aron, E. N., Vallone, R. D., & Bator, R. J. (1997). The experimental
generation of interpersonal closeness: A procedure and some preliminary findings.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(4), 363-377.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167297234003
Aron, A., Paris, M., & Aron, E. N. (1995). Falling in love: Prospective studies of self-concept
change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), 1102-1112.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1102
Arriaga, X. B., & Agnew, C. R. (2001). Being committed: Affective, cognitive, and conative
components of relationship commitment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
27(9), 1190-1203. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167201279011
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for
confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language,
68(3), 255-278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
Barrett, F. S., Grimm, K. J., Robins, R. W., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., & Janata, P. (2010).
Music-evoked nostalgia: Affect, memory, and personality. Emotion, 10(3), 390-403.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019006
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497529. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
30
Bazzini, D. G., Stack, E. R., Martincin, P. D., & Davis, C. P. (2007). The effect of reminiscing
about laughter on relationship satisfaction. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 25-34.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-006-9045-6
Biskas, M., Juhl, J., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., & Saroglou, V. (2021). Nostalgia and
spirituality: The roles of self-continuity and meaning in life. Manuscript under review,
University of Sheffield, UK.
Braithwaite, S. R., Delevi, R., & Fincham, F. D. (2010). Romantic relationships and the physical
and mental health of college students. Personal Relationships, 17(1), 1-12.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2010.01248.x
Bucher, A., Neubauer, A. B., Voss, A., & Oetzbach, C. (2019). Together is better: Higher
committed relationships increase life satisfaction and reduce loneliness. Journal of
Happiness Studies, 20, 2445-2469. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-018-0057-1
Cheung, W. Y., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Hepper, E. G., Arndt, J., & Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M.
(2013). Back to the future: Nostalgia increases optimism. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 39(11), 1484-1496. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213499187
Cortes, K., Leith, S., & Wilson, A. (2018). Relationship satisfaction and the subjective distance
of past relational events. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 35(8), 1092-1117.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407517704721
Cramer, D. (2004). Emotional support, conflict, depression, and relationship satisfaction in a
romantic partner. The Journal of Psychology, 138(6), 532-542.
https://doi.org/10.3200/JRLP.138.6.532-542
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
31
Cramer, D., & Jowett, S. (2010). Perceived empathy, accurate empathy and relationship
satisfaction in heterosexual couples. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 27(3),
327-349. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407509348384
Davis, M. H., & Oathout, H. A. (1987). Maintenance of satisfaction in romantic relationships:
Empathy and relational competence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
53(2), 397-410. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.2.397
Dibble, J. L., Levine, T. R., & Park, H. S. (2012). The Unidimensional Relationship Closeness
Scale (URCS): Reliability and validity evidence for a new measure of relationship
closeness. Psychological Assessment, 24(3), 565-572. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026265
Dodd, E., Ismail, S., Christopher, G., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., & Cheston, R. (2021).
Nostalgic conversations: The co-production of an intervention package for people living
with dementia and their spouse. Dementia: The International Journal of Social Research
and Practice. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/14713012211047350
Drivdahl, S. B. & Hyman Jr., I. E. (2014). Fluidity in autobiographical memories: Relationship
memories sampled on two occasions. Memory, 22(8). 1071-1080.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2013.866683
Dunlop, W. L. (2019). Love as story, love as storytelling. Personal Relationships, 26, 114-136.
https://doi.org/10.1111/pere.12271
Dunlop, W. L., Bühler, J., Maghsoodi, A., Harake, N., Wilkinson, D., & McAdams, D. P.
(2020). The stories couples live by. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships.
Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407520969900
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
32
Enders, C. K., & Tofighi, D. (2007). Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel
models: A new look at an old issue. Psychological Methods, 12(2), 121-138.
https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.12.2.121
Evans, N. D., Reyes, J., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., & Fetterman, A. K. (2021). Mental
transportation mediates nostalgia’s psychological benefits. Cognition and Emotion, 35(1),
84-95. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2020.1806788
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2013). Statistical power analyses using G*
Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods,
41(4), 1149-1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
Fehr, B., Harasymchuk, C., & Sprecher, S. (2014). Compassionate love in romantic
relationships: A review and some new findings. Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships, 31(5), 575-600. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407514533768
Fetterman, A. K., Wilkowski, B. M., & Robinson, M. D. (2018). On feeling warm and being
warm: Daily perceptions of physical warmth fluctuate with interpersonal warmth. Social
Psychological and Personality Science, 9(5), 560-567.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617712032
Fincham, F. D., & Linfield, K. J. (1997). A new look at marital quality: Can spouses feel positive
and negative about their marriage? Journal of Family Psychology, 11(4), 489502. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.11.4.489-502
Fincham, F. D., & Rogge, R. D. (2010). Understanding relationship quality: Theoretical
challenges and new tools for assessment. Journal of Family Theory and Review, 2(4),
227-242. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-2589.2010.00059.x
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
33
Fletcher, G. J., Simpson, J. A., & Thomas, G. (2000). The measurement of perceived relationship
quality components: A confirmatory factor analytic approach. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 26(3), 340-354. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167200265007
Frankenbach, J., Wildschut, T., Juhl, J., & Sedikides, C. (2021). Does neuroticism disrupt the
psychological benefits of nostalgia? A meta-analytic test. European Journal of
Personality, 35(2), 249-266. https://doi.org/10.1080/10.1002/per.2276
Franzoi, S. L., Davis, M. H., Young, R. D. (1985). The effects of private self-consciousness and
perspective taking on satisfaction in close relationships. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 48(6), 1584-1594. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.48.6.1584
Frost, D. M. (2013). The narrative construction of intimacy and affect in relationship stories:
Implications for relationship quality, stability, and mental health. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 30(3), 247-269. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407512454463
Gordon, A. M., Impett, E. A., Kogan, A., Oveis, C., & Keltner, D. (2012). To have and to hold:
Gratitude promotes relationship maintenance in intimate bonds. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 103(2), 257-274. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028723
Guilbault, V., & Philippe, F. L. (2017). Commitment in romantic relationships as a function of
partners’ encoding of couple-related memories. Memory, 25(5), 595-606.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2016.1197943
Gulledge, A. K., Gulledge, M. H., & Stahmann, R. F. (2003). Romantic physical affection types
and relationship satisfaction. The American Journal of Family Therapy, 31(4), 233-242.
https://doi.org/10.1080/01926180390201936
Hatfield, E., & Sprecher, S. (1986). Measuring passionate love in intimate relationships. Journal
of Adolescence, 9(4), 383-410. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-1971(86)80043-4
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
34
Hatfield, E., & Walster, G. W. (1978). A new look at love. University Press of America
Hendrick, C., & Hendrick (1986). A theory and method of love. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 50(2), 392-402. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.392
Hepper, E. G., Ritchie, T. D., Sedikides, C., & Wildschut, T. (2012). Odyssey’s end: Lay
conceptions of nostalgia reflect its original Homeric meaning. Emotion, 12(1), 102-119.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025167
Hepper, E. G., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Ritchie, T. D., Yung, Y.-F., Hansen, N.,
Abakoumkin, G., Arikan, G., Cisek, S. Z., Demassosso, D. B., Gebauer, J. E., Gerber, J.
P., González, R., Kusumi, T., Misra, G., Rusu, M., Ryan, O., Stephan, E., Vingerhoets,
A. J. J. M., & Zhou, X. (2014). Pancultural nostalgia: Prototypical conceptions across
cultures. Emotion, 14(4), 733-747. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036790
Hepper, E. G., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Robertson, S., & Routledge, C. D. (2021). Time
capsule: Nostalgia shields psychological wellbeing from limited time horizons. Emotion,
21(3), 644-664. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000728
Jostmann, N. B., Karremans, J., & Finkenauer, C. (2011). When love is not blind: Rumination
impairs affect regulation in response to romantic relationship threat. Cognition and
Emotion, 25(3), 506-518. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.541139
Juhl, J., Routledge, C., Arndt, J., Sedikides, C., & Wildschut, T. (2010). Fighting the future with
the past: Nostalgia buffers existential threat. Journal of Research in Personality, 44(3),
309-314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2010.02.006
Juhl, J., Sand, E. C., & Routledge, C. (2012). The effects of nostalgia and avoidant attachment on
relationship satisfaction and romantic motives. Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships, 29(5), 661-670. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407512443433
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
35
Juhl, J., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Diebel, T., Cheung, W. Y., & Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M.
(2020). Nostalgia proneness and empathy: Generality, underlying mechanism, and
implications for prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality, 88(3), 485-500.
https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12505
Juhl, J., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Xiong, X., & Zhou, X. (2021). Nostalgia promotes help
seeking by fostering social connectedness. Emotion, 21(3), 631-643.
https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000720
Karremans, J. C., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2004). Back to caring after being hurt: The role of
forgiveness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34(1), 207-227.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.192
Kito, M. (2005). Self-disclosure in romantic relationships and friendships among American and
Japanese college students. The Journal of Social Psychology, 145(2), 127-140.
https://doi.org/10.3200/SOCP.145.2.127-140
Koelsch, S. (2013). Striking a chord in the brain: Neurophysiological correlates of music-evoked
positive emotions. In T. Cochrane, B. Fantini, & K. R. Sherer (Eds.), Series in affective
science. The emotional power of music: Multidisciplinary perspectives on musical
arousal, expression, and social control (pp. 227-249). Oxford University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199654888.003.0017
Layous, K., Kurtz, J. L., Wildschut, T., & Sedikides, C. (2021). The effect of a multi-week
nostalgia intervention on well-being: Mechanisms and moderation. Emotion. Advance
online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000817
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
36
Lemieux, R., & Hale, J. L. (1999). Intimacy, passion, and commitment in young romantic
relationships: Successfully measuring the triangular theory of love. Psychological
Reports, 85(2), 497-503. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1999.85.2.497
Lenton, A. P., Slabu, L., & Sedikides, C. (2016). State authenticity in everyday life. European
Journal of Personality, 30(1), 64-82. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2033
Leunissen, J. M., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., & Routledge, C. (2021). The hedonic character of
nostalgia: An integrative data analysis. Emotion Review, 13(2), 139-156.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073920950455
Madoglou, A., Gkinopoulos, T., Xanthopoulos, P., & Kalamaras, D. (2017). Representations of
autobiographical nostalgic memories: Generational effect, gender, nostalgia proneness
and communication of nostalgic experiences. Journal of Integrated Social Sciences, 7,
60-88.
Mallory, A. B., Spencer, C. M., Kimmes, J. G., & Pollitt, A. M. (2018). Remembering the good
times: The influence of relationship nostalgia on relationship satisfaction across time.
Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 44(4), 561-574.
https://doi.org/10.1111/jmft.12311
Mattingly, B. A., McIntyre, K. P., & Lewandowski, G. W. (2020). Relationship-induced selfconcept change: Theoretical perspectives and methodological approaches. In B. A.
Mattingly, K. P. McIntyre, & G. W. Lewandowski (Eds.), Interpersonal relationships
and the self-concept (pp. 1-19). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-43747-3_1
Mattson, R. E., Rogge, R. D., Johnson, M. D., Davidson, E. K. B., & Fincham, F. D. (2013). The
positive and negative semantic dimensions of relationship satisfaction. Personal
Relationships, 20(2), 328-355. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2012.01412.x
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
37
Maxwell, G. M. (1985). Behaviour of lovers: Measuring closeness of relationships. Journal of
Social and Personal Relationships, 2(2), 215-238.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407585022007
McAdams, D. P. (1996). Personality, modernity, and the storied self: A contemporary framework
for studying persons. Psychological Inquiry, 7(4), 295-321.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0704_1
McAdams, D. P., Reynolds, J., Lewis, M., Patten, A. H., & Bowman, P. J. (2001). When bad
things turn good and good things turn bad: Sequences of redemption and contamination
in life narratives and their relation to psychosocial adaptation in midlife adults and in
students. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(4), 474–485.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167201274008
Muise, A., Kim, J. J., Debrot, A., Impett, E. A., & MacDonald, G. (2020). Sexual nostalgia as a
response to unmet sexual and relational needs: The role of attachment avoidance.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 46(11), 1538-1552.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167220907468
Murray, A. J., & Hazelwood, Z. J. (2011). Being grateful: Does it bring us closer? Gratitude,
attachment and intimacy in romantic relationships. Journal of Relationships Research,
2(1), 17-25. https://doi.org/10.1375/jrr.2.1.17
Neto, F. (2021). Estimates about love for self, romantic partners, and parents. Marriage &
Family Review, 57(2), 111-125. https://doi.org/10.1080/01494929.2020.1740371
Neto, F. & Wilks, D. C. (2017). Compassionate love for a romantic partner across the adult life
span. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 13(4), 606-617.
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v13i4.1204
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
38
Ohly, S., Sonnentag, S., Niessen, C., & Zapf, D. (2010). Diary studies in organizational research:
An introduction and some practical recommendations. Journal of Personnel Psychology,
9(2), 79-93. https://doi.org/10.1027/1866-5888/a000009
Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D., & R Core Team (2020). nlme: Linear and
nonlinear mixed effects models. R package version 3.1-148, https://CRAN.Rproject.org/package=nlme.
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data
analysis methods (Vol. 1). Sage.
Routledge, C., Arndt, J., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Hart, C. M., Juhl, J., Vingerhoets, A. J. J.
M., & Schlotz, W. (2011). The past makes the present meaningful: Nostalgia as an
existential resource. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(3), 638652. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024292
Routledge, C., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., & Juhl, J. (2013). Nostalgia as a resource for
psychological health and well‐being. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7(11),
808-818. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12070
Rusbult, C. E. (1980). Commitment and satisfaction in romantic associations: A test of the
investment model. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16(2) 172-186.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(80)90007-4
Rusbult, C. E. (1983). A longitudinal test of the investment model: The development (and
deterioration) of satisfaction and commitment in heterosexual involvements. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 45(1), 101-117. https://doi.org/10.1037/00223514.45.1.101
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
39
Rusbult, C. E., & Buunk, B. P. (1993). Commitment processes in close relationships: An
interdependence analysis. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10(2), 175-204.
https://doi.org/10.1177/026540759301000202
Rusbult, C. E., Hannon, P. A., Stocker, S. L., & Finkel, E. J. (2005). Forgiveness and relational
repair. In E. L. Worthington Jr. (Ed.), Handbook of forgiveness (pp. 185-205). Routledge.
Rusbult, C. E., Martz, J. M., & Agnew, C. R. (1998). The investment model scale: Measuring
commitment level, satisfaction level, quality of alternatives, and investment size.
Personal Relationships, 5(4), 357-391. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.14756811.1998.tb00177.x
Schönbrodt, F. D., & Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do correlations stabilize? Journal
of Research in Personality, 47(1), 609-612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.05.009
Schröder-Abé, M., & Schütz, A. (2011). Walking in each other’s shoes: Perspective taking
mediates effects of emotional intelligence on relationship quality. European Journal of
Personality, 25(2), 155-169. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.818
Sedikides, C., Campbell, W. K., Reeder, G. D., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). The relationship closeness
induction task. Representative Research in Social Psychology, 23, 1-4.
Sedikides, C., Oliver, M. B., & Campbell, W. K. (1994). Perceived benefits and costs of
romantic relationships for women and men: Implications for exchange theory. Personal
Relationships, 1(1), 5-21. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.1994.tb00052.x
Sedikides, C., & Wildschut, T. (2016). Nostalgia: A bittersweet emotion that confers
psychological health benefits. In A. M. Wood & J. Johnson (Eds.), Wiley handbook of
positive clinical psychology (pp. 25-36). Wiley.
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
40
Sedikides, C., & Wildschut, T. (2018). Finding meaning in nostalgia. Review of General
Psychology, 22(1), 48-61. https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000109
Sedikides C., & Wildschut, T. (2019). The sociality of personal and collective nostalgia.
European Review of Social Psychology, 30(1), 123-173.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2019.1630098
Sedikides, C., Wildschut, T., Cheung, W.-Y., Routledge, C., Hepper, E. G., Arndt, J., Vail, K.,
Zhou, X., Brackstone, K., & Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M. (2016). Nostalgia fosters selfcontinuity: Uncovering the mechanism (social connectedness) and the consequence
(eudaimonic well-being). Emotion, 16(4), 524-539. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000136
Sedikides, C., Wildschut, T., Routledge, C., Arndt, J., Hepper, E. G., & Zhou, X. (2015). To
nostalgize: Mixing memory with affect and desire. Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology, 51, 189-273. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2014.10.001
Sedikides, C., Wildschut, T., & Stephan, E. (2018). Nostalgia shapes and potentiates the future.
In J. P. Forgas & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), The social psychology of living well (pp. 181199). Routledge.
Simpson, J. A., & Campbell, L. (Eds.) (2013). The Oxford handbook of close relationships.
Oxford University Press.
Sprecher, S., & Fehr, B. (2005). Compassionate love for close others and humanity. Journal of
Social and Personal Relationships, 22(5), 629-651.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407505056439
Stephan, E., Sedikides, C., Wildschut, T., Cheung, W. Y., Routledge, C., & Arndt, J. (2015).
Nostalgia-evoked inspiration: Mediating mechanisms and motivational implications.
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(10), 1395-1410.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167215596985
Steiger, J. H. (2004). Beyond the F test: Effect size confidence intervals and tests of close fit in
the analysis of variance and contrast analysis. Psychological Methods, 9(2), 164-182.
https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.9.2.164
Sternberg, R. J., & Grajek, S. (1984). The nature of love. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 47(2), 312-329. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.47.2.312
The New Oxford Dictionary of English. (1998). (J. Pearsall, Ed.). Oxford University Press.
Van Tilburg, W. A. P., Wildschut, T., & Sedikides, C. (2018). Nostalgia’s place among selfrelevant emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 32(4), 742-759.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2017.1351331
Van Tilburg, W. A. P., Bruder, M., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., & Göritz, A. S. (2019). An
appraisal profile of nostalgia. Emotion, 19(1), 21-36.
https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000417
Vess, M., Arndt, J., Routledge, C., Sedikides, C., & Wildschut, T. (2012). Nostalgia as a
resource for the self. Self and Identity, 11(3), 273-284.
https://doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2010.521452
Wildschut, T., Bruder, M., Robertson, S., Van Tilburg, A. P. W., & Sedikides, C. (2014).
Collective nostalgia: A group-level emotion that confers unique benefits on the group.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(5), 844-863.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037760
41
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
42
Wildschut, T., & Sedikides, C. (2022). Psychology and nostalgia: Towards a functional
approach. In M. H. Jacobsen (Ed.), Intimations of nostalgia: Multidisciplinary
explorations of an enduring emotion (pp. 110-128). Bristol University Press.
Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Arndt, J., & Routledge, C. (2006). Nostalgia: Content, triggers,
functions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(5), 975-993.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.975
Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Routledge, C., Arndt, J., & Cordaro, F. (2010). Nostalgia as a
repository of social connectedness: The role of attachment-related avoidance. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 98(4), 573-586. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017597
Zhou, X., Sedikides, C., Wildschut, T., Gao, D.-G. (2008). Counteracting loneliness: On the
restorative function of nostalgia. Psychological Science, 19(10), 1023-1029.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02194.x
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
Table 1
Correlations Between Romantic Nostalgia and Relational Benefits in
Study 1
Variable
1
1. Romantic Nostalgia
2
3
4
-
2. Closeness
.23***
-
3. Satisfaction
.21***
.81***
-
4. Commitment
.15*
.78***
.62***
5. Relationship Length
-.06
.06
.02
Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
.24***
43
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
44
Table 2
Effect of Romantic Nostalgia on Relational Benefits in Study 2
Condition M(SD), n
Feeling/Experience
Nostalgia
Control
F
df
η²
90% CI
1. Closeness
6.41(0.64), 54
6.15(0.80), 76
3.80
1,128 .029 .000, .092
2. Satisfaction
6.19(0.70), 57
5.77(1.14), 76
5.93*
1,131 .043 .004, .113
3. Commitment
6.23(0.87), 57
5.77(1.00), 75
7.89**
1,130 .057 .010, .133
Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
45
Table 3
Correlations Among Relational Benefits and Relationship Duration in Study 3
Variable
1
1. Connectedness
-
2
3
4
5
6
2. Optimism
.90***
-
3. Closeness
.27**
.29***
-
4. Satisfaction
.32***
.31***
.67***
-
5. Commitment
.13
.17*
.67***
.61***
-
6. Compassionate Love
.26**
.29***
.63***
.47***
.51***
-
7. Passionate Love
.29***
.31***
.63***
.48***
.51***
.58***
-.18*
.00
-.11
.13
-.01
8. Relationship Duration -.12
Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
7
-.10
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
46
Table 4
Effect of Romantic Nostalgia on Relational Benefits in Study 3
Condition M(SD), n
Feeling/Experience
Nostalgia
Control
F
df
η²
90% CI
1. Connectedness
91.19(14.19), 75
31.16(29.99), 76
246.03*** 1,149 .623 .548, .682
2. Optimism
91.48(10.72), 75
39.25(31.22), 75
187.80*** 1,148 .559 .476, .627
3. Closeness
90.63(10.31), 75
85.45(13.75), 75
6.805*
1,148 .044 .006, .110
4. Satisfaction
88.22(13.16), 75
81.34(16.49), 76
8.01**
1,149 .051 .009, .119
5. Commitment
90.69(11.94), 72
87.24(17.04), 75
2.00
1,145 .014 .000, .061
6. Compassionate Love
89.57(9.51), 75
85.77(11.29), 76
5.018*
1,149 .033 .002, .092
7. Passionate Love
79.48(11.58), 75
73.16(18.28), 76
6.416*
1,149 .041 .005, .105
Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
ROMANTIC NOSTALGIA AND RELATIONSHIP FUNCTIONING
47
Table 5
Multilevel Models Testing the Unique Association between Daily Romantic Nostalgia and Daily Relational Benefits in Study 4
Model No.
Model 1
Model 2
Outcome
Predictor
b
SE
t
df
p
95% CI
Connectedness
Romantic Nostalgia
.229
.031
7.33
925
<.001
.168, .290
Optimism
Romantic Nostalgia
.141
.029
4.82
925
<.001
.084, .199
Desire to Leave
Romantic Nostalgia
-.089
.026
-3.44
925
<.001
-.140. -.038
Connectedness
Time Spent with Partner
.294
.031
9.45
876
<.001
.233, .355
Romantic Nostalgia
.159
.029
5.51
876
<.001
.103, .216
Time Spent with Partner
.147
.029
4.99
876
<.001
.089, .205
Romantic Nostalgia
.109
.030
3.63
876
<.001
.050, .167
Time Spent with Partner
-.047
.029
-1.61
876
.108
-.105, .010
Romantic Nostalgia
-.086
.025
-3.46
876
<.001
-.135, -.037
Optimism
Desire to Leave