RESEARCH ARTICLE
Increased Cerebral Water Content in
Hemodialysis Patients
Kathrin Reetz1,2,3☯*, Zaheer Abbas1,2,3☯, Ana Sofia Costa1,3, Vincent Gras2, Frances TiffinRichards1,3, Shahram Mirzazade1,2,3, Bernhard Holschbach4, Rolf Dario Frank5,
Athina Vassiliadou6, Thilo Krüger7¤a, Frank Eitner7¤a, Theresa Gross7¤b, Jörg
Bernhard Schulz1,3, Jürgen Floege7, Nadim Jon Shah1,2,3
a11111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Reetz K, Abbas Z, Costa AS, Gras V, TiffinRichards F, Mirzazade S, et al. (2015) Increased
Cerebral Water Content in Hemodialysis Patients.
PLoS ONE 10(3): e0122188. doi:10.1371/journal.
pone.0122188
Academic Editor: Su Lui, West China Hospital of
Sichuan University, CHINA
Received: August 6, 2014
Accepted: February 10, 2015
Published: March 31, 2015
Copyright: © 2015 Reetz et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits
unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any
medium, provided the original author and source are
credited.
Data Availability Statement: All relevant data are
within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Nevertheless, the correspondence author Dr. Kathrin
Reetz can be contacted at
[email protected] for
further questions.
Funding: Sources of support: This work was
supported in part by the Excellence Initiative of the
German Research Foundation [DFG ZUK32/1] and
the German Federal Ministry of Education and
Research [BMBF 01GQ1402] to KR; and by a PhD
fellowship (SFRH/BD/65743 2009) from the
Fundação para a Ciência e Tecnologia (FCT),
financed by the POPH – QREN Program to ASC.
1 Department of Neurology, RWTH Aachen University Hospital, Germany, 2 Institute of Neuroscience and
Medicine (INM-4), Research Centre Jülich GmbH, Jülich, Germany, 3 Jülich Aachen Research Alliance
(JARA)—Translational Brain Medicine, Jülich and Aachen, Germany, 4 KfH Kuratorium für Dialyse und
Nierentransplantation e.V., Stolberg, Germany, 5 Department of Internal Medicine, St.-Antonius-Hospital
Eschweiler, Eschweiler, Germany, 6 Dialysezentrum Aachen, Praxis und Dialyse, Aachen, Germany,
7 Division of Nephrology and Clinical Immunology, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
☯ These authors contributed equally to this work.
¤a Current address: Bayer Pharma AG, Global Drug Discovery, Kidney Diseases Research, Wuppertal,
Germany
¤b Current address: Department of Internal Medicine, Dresden-Friedreichstadt Hospital, Dresden, Germany
*
[email protected]
Abstract
Little information is available on the impact of hemodialysis on cerebral water homeostasis
and its distribution in chronic kidney disease. We used a neuropsychological test battery,
structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and a novel technique for quantitative measurement of localized water content using 3T MRI to investigate ten hemodialysis patients
(HD) on a dialysis-free day and after hemodialysis (2.4±2.2 hours), and a matched healthy
control group with the same time interval. Neuropsychological testing revealed mainly attentional and executive cognitive dysfunction in HD. Voxel-based-morphometry showed only
marginal alterations in the right inferior medial temporal lobe white matter in HD compared
to controls. Marked increases in global brain water content were found in the white matter,
specifically in parietal areas, in HD patients compared to controls. Although the global water
content in the gray matter did not differ between the two groups, regional increases of brain
water content in particular in parieto-temporal gray matter areas were observed in HD patients. No relevant brain hydration changes were revealed before and after hemodialysis.
Whereas longer duration of dialysis vintage was associated with increased water content in
parieto-temporal-occipital regions, lower intradialytic weight changes were negatively correlated with brain water content in these areas in HD patients. Worse cognitive performance
on an attention task correlated with increased hydration in frontal white matter. In conclusion, long-term HD is associated with altered brain tissue water homeostasis mainly in parietal white matter regions, whereas the attentional domain in the cognitive dysfunction
profile in HD could be linked to increased frontal white matter water content.
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
1 / 18
Quantitative Cerebral Water Mapping in Hemodialysis
NJS and JBS were in part funded by the Helmholtz
Alliance ICEMED - Imaging and Curing
Environmental Metabolic Diseases [HA-314], through
the Initiative and Network Fund of the Helmholtz
Association. The sponsors had no role in the study
design, data collection, data analysis, data
interpretation, or writing of the report. Bayer Pharma
AG provided support for the authors TK and FE, but
did not have any additional role in the study design,
data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or
preparation of the manuscript. The specific roles of
these authors are articulated in the ‘author
contributions’ section.
Competing Interests: The authors have the
following interests. This study is partly supported by
the Helmholtz Alliance ICEMED. Thilo Krüger and
Frank Eitner are now employed by Bayer Pharma
AG. There are no patents, products in development
or marketed products to declare. This does not alter
the authors' adherence to all the PLOS ONE policies
on sharing data and materials, as detailed online in
the guide for authors.
Introduction
In chronic kidney disease (CKD) patients, cognitive impairment is highly prevalent and increases as kidney function worsens [1, 2]. In patients undergoing hemodialysis (HD), it
markedly exceeds values observed in age-matched controls [3, 4]. Knowledge of cerebral
changes in CKD, as well as the impact of HD on cognition and brain function is limited. Although the brain and the kidneys both show a similar vulnerability to vascular damage [5],
their pathophysiological linkage is multifaceted. Both suffer damage from hypertension, diabetes, hypercholesterolemia, anemia and inflammatory changes [6–8] resulting in an increased
prevalence of silent brain infarcts as well as microbleeds in CKD patients [8]. Besides cerebrovascular changes, a direct uremic neuronal toxicity supports additional neurodegenerative
pathways [9]. Although the cerebrovascular aspect is also supported by the neuropsychological
pattern of cognitive impairment in CKD patients, namely executive functions and psychomotor speed deficits [10], memory-relevant alterations have also been reported [11]. Furthermore,
cognitive functions fluctuate over the course of the dialysis cycle and cognitive functioning is
mostly vulnerable during the dialysis session [12]. This finding suggests that direct consequences of CKD such as overhydration and/or the uremic milieu may also affect cognitive
functioning [13].
Non-invasive, quantitative mapping of water content in the in vivo human brain has demonstrated the ability to provide important disease-related parameters associated with focal or
global change in tissue water homeostasis [14]. We used a recently validated magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) technique for quantitative water content measurement [15, 16] to investigate brain water changes in HD patients.
The aim of the study was to identify such changes in late stage CKD and possible variations
during the dialysis cycle, as well as their relation to clinical disease-specific features using a recently validated whole-brain MRI sequence.
Methods
Participants
Ten patients undergoing hemodialysis in the greater Aachen area were recruited (for detailed
characteristics of the study sample please see Table 1). The control group comprised ten
healthy volunteers with no history of cardiovascular or kidney diseases matched for age, gender
and education. Exclusion criteria for all study participants included contraindications for research MRI, severe sensory impairment, and previous history of neurological or psychiatric
disease. All subjects provided written informed consent for participation in this study, which
was approved by the local ethics committee of the RWTH Aachen University (EK 179/11) and
conducted in accordance to the latest version of the Helsinki declaration.
MRI and neuropsychological testing were applied at two time points: (t1) dialysis-free day
and (t2) after dialysis (mean 2.4 hours ± 2.2 standard deviation [SD] time after dialysis session
for MRI measurement). Eight of ten patients were retested within the same dialysis cycle
(24-hours time period), but due to acute intercurrent illness one patient was retested within
48 hours and one patient within one month. Healthy controls were all retested within 24 hours.
All participants received a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment including major
cognitive domains—attention, memory, executive functions, visuospatial processing and language—, as well as screening instruments for depression and anxiety (HADS, Hospital Anxiety
and Depression Scale [17]), daytime sleepiness (Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS)) and fatigue
[18], as previously described by Costa et al, [19]. The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)
[20] and the Montreal Cognitive Examination (MoCA) [21, 22] were used as brief cognitive
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
2 / 18
Quantitative Cerebral Water Mapping in Hemodialysis
Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the hemodialysis patients and healthy control groups.
Demographic and clinical variables
Hemodialysis patients
n = 10
Healthy controls
n = 10
p-value
Age, years
45.80 (12.67)
50.30 (14.44)
.47
Sex, % female
40
40
1
Education, years
12.00 (3.06)
14.00 (2.16)
.11
Diabetes
20%
none
.47
Hypertension
90%
20%
<.05
CV risk factors
Hypercholesterolaemia
10%
none
1
Smoking, (py)
6.3 (10.2)
0
.24
BMI (km/m2)
.68
24.7 (4.4)
23.6 (3.3)
Cardiovascular Risk (SCORE)
1 (1.5)
-
-
Charlson Comorbidity Index (CCI)
3.1 (1.7)
.80 (1.1)
<.01
Medication
Antihypertensives
90%
20%
<.05
Insulin
20%
none
.47
Thyroid drugs
30%
none
.21
Antipsychotics
20%
none
.47
Antidepressants
20%
none
.47
Anxiolytics
10%
none
1
Analgesics
10%
none
1
Levodopa
10%
none
1
n affecting cognition
30%
none
.32
Diabetic nephropathy
10%
-
-
Glomerulonephritis and systemic diseases
20%
-
-
Primary cause of CKD
Polycystic kidney disease
20%
-
-
Other/Unknown
20%/30%
-
-
CKD associated comorbidities
Renal anemia
80%
-
-
Secondary hyperparathyroidism
20%
-
-
Renovascular hypertension
40%
-
-
History of kidney transplant
10%
-
-
Dialysis parameter
Median dialysis vintage, months
44.50
-
-
Median intradialytic weight change, kg
-1.5
-
-
Intradialytic hypotensive episodes
.44 (1.0)
-
-
Hematocrit (l/l)
.325 (.05)
-
-
Hemoglobin (g/dl)
10.6 (1.9)
-
-
Platelets (G/l)
282.3 (90.3)
-
-
Leukocytes (G/l)
7.8 (2.0)
-
-
Sodium (mmol/l)
137.3 (3.2)
-
-
Potassium (mmol/l)
4.8 (1.3)
-
-
Urea (mg/dl)
136.9 (59.0)
-
Blood values
(Continued)
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
3 / 18
Quantitative Cerebral Water Mapping in Hemodialysis
Table 1. (Continued)
Demographic and clinical variables
Creatinine (mg/dl)
Hemodialysis patients
n = 10
Healthy controls
n = 10
p-value
9.7 (4.1)
-
-
All data shown as mean (SD), except where noted. Charlson Comorbidity Index (CCI) corrected for dialysis patients and corrected for age in the control
group. CV = Cerebrovascular; py = pack years; CKD = Chronic kidney disease. Other causes include progression of CKD due to post-operative infections,
reflux diseases, analgesic medication.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188.t001
screening tools to assess global cognition. Cued and not-cued reaction times were measured
through the Alertness sub-test of the Test of Attentional Performance [23]. The Trail making
test (TMT) parts A and B [24] provided an index on processing speech and cognitive flexibility.
We measured the span of immediate verbal recall and auditory working memory through the
digit span forwards and backwards [25]. The verbal fluency task [26], with semantic and phonemic modalities assesses functions related to language production, as well as executive functions. The Stroop test [27] was used as a measure of selective attention and inhibitory control.
The California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) test [28] was used for verbal episodic memory
providing measures on verbal learning, immediate and delayed recall, as well as recognition.
We used the MCGCF complex figure [29] to assess visual perception and construction (figure
copy), as well as visual memory (3-minutes immediate and 30-minutes delayed recall). To exclude basal language and visual perception deficits, participants completed at baseline a shortform of the Boston Naming Test [29] and the Incomplete Letters sub-test from the Visual Object Spatial Perception (VOSP) battery [30].
Given the repeated-measures nature of the study alternate-forms were used, when available,
and test order and time of assessment order were controlled for (for details [19]).
A detailed medical history for all patients with the following parameters was obtained: CKD
etiology, hemodialysis vintage duration (the duration in months of the hemodialysis treatment)
and volume of hemodialysis, frequency of intradialytic hypotensive episodes, as well as the following routine blood work: hematocrit, hemoglobin, leukocytes, platelets, glucose, sodium, potassium, urea, and creatinine. Comorbidities were quantified using the Charlson Comorbidity
Index (CCI), which predicts the ten-year mortality by weighting several comorbid conditions,
either corrected for dialysis [31] or corrected for age in the healthy control subjects [32]. The
presence of cardiovascular risk factors (CVRF)—diabetes, hypertension, hypercholesterolemia,
smoking, body mass index (BMI)—was recorded for all participants. For patients, CVRFs were
rated using the low SCORE risk charts [33]. Medication was also listed for all participants and
neuroleptics, antidepressants, opioids, as well as antiparkinsonian agents were considered as
psychoactive medication.
MRI data acquisition
MR measurements were performed on a 3 Tesla Trio MR scanner at the RWTH Aachen University (Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany). Radio frequency (RF) excitation and
signal reception were achieved with a body coil and a 32-channel receive phased array head
coil, respectively. Quantitative cerebral water content measurements were based on the estimation of the MR-visible proton density (PD) [14, 16, 34–40]. It is indeed well accepted that the
MR-visible proton density (PD) provides a very reliable measure of free water content present
in the tissue [41]. A rough estimate of the MR-visible PD was obtained using a standard multislice, multi-echo Gradient Recalled Echo (GRE) MRI sequence (TR = 1800ms, TE = 5.2ms and
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
4 / 18
Quantitative Cerebral Water Mapping in Hemodialysis
FA = 40°), below referred to as the PD scan. Subsequently, the following transformations were
applied on the PD scan prior to data analysis [16, 34, 40]: i) compensation of the transmit (B1+)
and receive field (B1-) inhomogeneity ii) compensation of the T2 decay and iii) correction of
the T1-saturation effect iv) correction of residual nonuniformity and v) normalization of the
image to the ventricular cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) signal to obtain the water molarity (mol/L)
of the tissue, expressed as a percentage of pure water molarity. The steps i) to iii) necessitate
the inclusion of additional MRI scans into the water content protocol, namely, a second GRE
scan (TR = 500ms, TE = 5.2ms and FA = 90°) for estimating T1 and applying the T1-saturation
correction, a series of four single-shot GRE-EPI scans (TE = 11ms and FA = 30°, 60°, 90°, 120°)
for estimating B1+ and a set of two low-resolution GRE scans (TR = 500ms, TE = 5.2ms and
FA = 40°) for obtaining B1- (Abbas et al., 2014). Finally, a multi-echo 3D-GRE MR acquisition
was performed (TR = 35ms, FA = 12°, TE1 = 2.3ms, ΔTE = 2.27ms, 8 echoes). It allows estimating the T2 decay constant and correct for the signal decay for the first echo image of the PDscan. The PD-scan consisted of 64 transverse slices, read in two interleaved concatenations (resulting in a gap free acquisition). The readout direction was anterior-posterior and the phase
field-of-view (FOV) was adjusted to contain the entire brain (typically 190 mm). The sequences
for mapping T1, T2 , B1+, B1- were adjusted to the same slice orientation and FOV as the PDscan. Parallel imaging was employed with the generalized auto-calibrating partially parallel acquisition (GRAPPA) technique [42] with an acceleration factor of two, which allowed shortening significantly the acquisition time. The total scan time for the water mapping protocol was
fourteen minutes.
In addition to the water content protocol, a high-resolution T1-weighted image was acquired using a magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo sequence (MPRAGE) with
TR = 1900ms, TE = 2.52ms, TI = 900ms, FoV = 250mm, 256x256 matrix, 176 sagittal slices,
slice thickness = 1mm, resulting in an acquisition time of five minutes.
Voxel-Based Morphometry (VBM) analysis
To investigate the structural differences between HD patients and controls, the MPRAGE datasets were analyzed using the Statistical Parametric Mapping toolbox (SPM8, www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.
uk/spm) and the voxel-based-morphometry toolbox (VBM8, http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/
vbm). Applying a standard VBM approach [43], the MPRAGE images were spatially normalized by high-dimensional warping with a standard template and segmented into gray matter
(GM), white matter (WM) and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF). To correct for individual brain sizes
and to allow the comparison of the absolute amount of tissue volume, voxel values were multiplied (modulated) by the non-linear component of the Jacobian determinant derived from the
spatial normalization. Finally, modulated gray matter and white matter images were smoothed
with a kernel of 12 mm Gaussian full width at half maximum (FWHM). Gray matter and white
matter differences between patients and controls were tested using two-sample t-tests, including age as a nuisance covariate. Results were accessed at highest threshold of P < 0.05 (family
wise error [FWE]-corrected) across the whole brain and by using minimum cluster size (kE) of
80 voxels. Coordinates are reported in the standard anatomical space developed at the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI).
Global and focal analyses of quantitative brain water content
To analyze cerebral water content changes within the brain, the MPRAGE and the quantitative
PD and T1 images delivered by the quantitative water content protocol were normalized to
MNI template for each individual (1mm isotropic MNI152 T1–weighted), using the FSL-tool
FLIRT (http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl), with a 12-parameter affine transformation [44].
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
5 / 18
Quantitative Cerebral Water Mapping in Hemodialysis
Subsequently, the MPRAGE image was segmented into gray and white matter as well as cerebrospinal fluid using SPM8 segmentation [45]. In order to efficiently suppress the influence of
partial volume effects, all voxels with gray and white matter as well as cerebrospinal fluid probabilities smaller than one were set to zero [46].
Histograms of the MR parameters of interest (PD and T1) were computed after brain volume segmentation. In order to estimate the PD and T1 values in white matter and gray matter,
discriminant analyses for the bivariate histogram of (PD, T1) were performed using the MIXMOD software package which allows performing model-based classifications of qualitative and
quantitative data (http://mixmod.org) [47]. Furthermore, global differences between HD patients and healthy controls groups were assessed using non-parametric Wilcoxon ranksum test.
For the purpose of focal sub-analyses, several ROIs were selected from the MNI standard
template (probability threshold of 75% or above was used for masking) to compute the mean
values and standard deviations of water content and T1, separately for white matter and gray
matter. The following ROIs, obtained from the MNI atlas [48], Harvard-Oxford cortical and
subcortical structural atlas [49–52], were used for further screening and partitioning in an exploratory manner to quantify cerebral water content: total intracranial volume, total white matter, total gray matter, frontal, temporal, parietal and occipital cortices, caudate, putamen,
thalamus, globus pallidus, para- and hippocampus, corpus callosum (body), insula, brainstem,
cingulum (anterior cingulate, angular cortex, posterior cingulate cortex), cerebellum, superior
frontal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus (pars triangularis and pars opercularis), superior temporal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, inferior temporal gyrus, superior parietal lobule gyrus, operculum (frontal, central and parietal), precuneus, lateral occipital cortex
(superior, inferior division), supracalcarine cortex and occipital pole.
In order to report water content from mixed gray and white matter brain structures (e.g.,
angular gyrus with 50% gray matter and 49% white matter), the atlas-based ROI masks were
multiplied by the subject specific gray or white matter mask so as to report white matter or
gray matter water content value for such region. This analysis concerned only structures having
more than 7% gray or white matter tissues. Only significant changes in cerebral tissue water
homeostasis are reported.
Statistical analysis
Depending on the comparison and respective test assumptions, group differences based on demographic and clinical variables were reported using the independent-sample T test, MannWhitney or the X2 test. Accounting for missing data, we calculated linear mixed models with
random effects for correlated data, with education and testing order as covariates, to determine
group, time and interaction of group and time effects on cognitive performance and in the regional water content. The goodness-of-fit and the normality of the residuals were verified. Association measures between clinical and cognitive data (at baseline) and water mapping data
were performed by computing Person’s (r) or Spearman’s [53] correlation coefficients, with a
significance threshold of 0.05. Effect sizes were calculated using Cohen’s d test [54] (0.3 small,
0.5 medium, 0.9 to infinity large). Bonferroni correction was used accordingly to correct for
multiple comparisons.
Results
Neuropsychological profile
Neuropsychological performance of HD patients and healthy controls, before and after dialysis,
are reported in Table 2, HD patients performed consistently worse than matched healthy
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
6 / 18
Quantitative Cerebral Water Mapping in Hemodialysis
Table 2. Neuropsychological performance of hemodialysis patients and healthy controls on t1 and t2 time-points.
Cognitive Domain/Test
Hemodialysis Patients (n = 10)
Healthy Controls (n = 10)
t1
Mean (SD)
t2
Mean (SD)
t1
Mean (SD)
t2
Mean (SD)
Mixed Model
MoCA (total score)
24.00 (2.67)
24.00 (4.0)
26.90 (1.97)
27.7 (1.25)
g**
MMSE (total score)
24.57 (10.8)
28.83 (1.17)
29.00 (.87)
28.98 (1.45)
ns
Screening
Attention
Phasic Alertness (msec.)
248.99 (38.70)
286.75 (59.87)
240.00 (35.49)
228.8 (36.49)
i*
Intrinsic Alertness (msec.)
251.13 (32.19)
290.00 (54.40)
231.00 (24.89)
218.50 (26.42)
i*
Verbal Memory
Digit span forwards
6.86 (1.07)
7.00 (1.55)
8.60 (1.84)
7.00 (1.55)
g*
CVLT Total learning
51.71 (5.16)
51.67 (9.67)
55.10 (10.65)
52.80 (13.05)
ns
CVLT Interference
5.14 (1.68)
4.67 (2.42)
6.50 (2.76)
6.40 (2.46)
ns
CVLT Immediate recall
10.29 (3.15)
9.17 (3.32)
12.10 (3.25)
11.70 (3.16)
i**
CVLT Delayed recall
11.00 (2.08)
10.5 (3.02)
12.60 (2.50)
12.00 (3.13)
ns
CVLT Recognition
15.57 (.79)
15.67 (.52)
15.60 (.52)
15.60 (.69)
ns
Complex Figure Immediate recall
20.86 (6.29)
22.00 (6.36)
26.55 (6.65)
23.39 (9.28)
ns
Complex Figure delayed recall
19.33 (6.38)
21.50 (6.89)
24.39 (8.14)
23.28 (9.99)
ns
Complex Figure Copy
31.14 (2.73)
31.33 (2.81)
33.50 (2.32)
33.0 (2.28)
g*
VOSP Incomplete Letters
19.57 (.79)
-
19.00 (.83)
-
ns
14.43 (.79)
-
14.90 (.32)
-
ns
Nonverbal Memory
Visuospatial functions
Language
Boston Naming Test
Executive Functions
Digit span backwards
5.86 (.90)
5.67 (1.63)
6.70 (1.16)
5.67 (1.63)
g**
Semantic verbal fluency
25.43 (7.44)
25.00 (8.69)
33.70 (6.06)
31.00 (8.86)
g**
Phonemic verbal fluency
11.57 (4.54)
12.50 (1.52)
22.60 (7.53)
22.30 (8.83)
g***
TMT A (sec.)
54.29 (30.38)
46.5 (22.6)
28.40 (8.48)
24.70 (6.95)
g*
TMT B (sec.)
141.33 (94.58)
170.5 (132.73)
60.9 (14.29)
66.10 (32.15)
g*
Stroop Reading (sec.)
34.83 (10.84)
37.00 (12.02)
31.10 (3.07)
29.70 (4.08)
ns
Stroop Naming (sec.)
60.17 (15.07)
56.00 (10.67)
49.4 (8.21)
46.70 (8.37)
ns
Stroop Interference (sec.)
115.67 (50.47)
116.17 (58.46)
82.5 (19.10)
75.10 (15.24)
ns
HADS
Anxiety
5.67 (4.58)
-
1.40 (1.58)
-
ns
Depression
2.78 (2.49)
-
.70 (1.25)
-
§**
Fatigue
5.17 (2.40)
5.67 (1.75)
.75 (.96)
.50 (.58)
g***
Sleepiness (ESS)
7.00 (2.49)
-
5.90 (3.32)
-
ns
Note. MoCA total score corresponds to the total uncorrected score; MoCA = Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MMSE = Mini-mental State Examination,
CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test, VOSP = Visual Object and Space Perception Battery, TMT = Trail Making Test, HADS = Hospital Anxiety and
Depression Scale; ESS = Epworth Sleepiness Scale. ns, not significant; i, significant interaction of group and time; g, significant main effect of group; §
significant Wilcoxon-Mann–Whitney U test, with Bonferroni correction for multiple testing (p <.01). Mixed linear model Group x Time, with education and
presentation order as covariates.
* p <. 05
** p <. 01
*** p <. 001.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188.t002
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
7 / 18
Quantitative Cerebral Water Mapping in Hemodialysis
controls in several cognitive domains, especially in attention (phasic and intrinsic alertness,
digit span) and executive functions (verbal fluency, working memory, cognitive flexibility). We
found no differences between groups regarding verbal and nonverbal memory functions, nor
basal language and visuospatial functioning. We found an interaction effect of dialysis on the
phasic and intrinsic alertness and immediate verbal recall performance, where patients showed
a decline in performance after dialysis, whereas controls showed an improvement in performance at re-test. Patients presented with higher levels of fatigue, which remained stable around
the dialysis cycle. They also scored significantly higher for anxiety and total HADS score when
compared to controls.
Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) of white and gray matter
VBM analysis revealed no significant differences in gray matter and white matter volume when
comparing HD patients to healthy controls, apart from one cluster of 96 voxels in the white
matter of the right inferior medial temporal lobe (x = 34, y = 4, z = -36, Z = 4.78). There were
no differences in gray and white matter between measurements t1 and t2. However, total brain
tissue volume was 7% lower in HD patients compared to controls.
Brain water content analysis
The quantitative water content maps (Fig 1) (an overlaid p-value map on a MNI template) and
the histograms (Fig 2) show that the spatial distribution of the estimated brain water content
was altered predominantly in the white matter (p = 0.03, d = 0.98) in HD patients as compared
to controls (Table 3). Whereas increased brain water content in white matter was found in the
frontal (p = 0.03, d = 0.96), temporal (p = 0.04, d = 0.90), parietal (p = 0.02, d = 1.14) and occipital (p = 0.03, d = 0.95) lobes, gray matter water content was only affected in temporal
(p = 0.04, d = 0.56) and parietal lobes (p = 0.04, d = 0.94). No within-group changes in brain
hydration were found for the HD and control groups before and after dialysis. Whereas, greater
brain water content values in HD patients compared to controls group were found in the largest white matter bundle, the corpus callosum. Regarding the frontal lobe, increased brain water
content was found in the frontal white matter (inferior frontal gyrus, operculum cortex) and
gray matter regions (frontal pole, inferior frontal gyrus pars triangularis). The white matter in
the temporal lobe showed increased water content in the superior temporal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, inferior temporal gyrus and the hippocampus as well as in gray matter tissue in the
superior temporal gyrus, fusiform cortex, temporal pole, and hippocampus. However, the
major increase of water content alterations was revealed in the parietal lobe (large effect size),
in the white matter (postcentral gyrus, parietal operculum, supramarginal gyrus, angular gyrus,
intracalcarine cortex) and in the gray matter (supramarginal gyrus, angular gyrus, parietal
operculum cortex, supracalcarine cortex). Increased occipital water content was found in the
lateral occipital cortex and in the occipital pole. In the remaining regions the differences between HD patients and controls were not statistically significant regarding time and group
using the mixed linear model.
Association with demographic, clinical parameters and cognitive
performance
Association measures showed that the duration of dialysis vintage (Fig 3A) was positively correlated with the water content observed in the parietal white matter (r = 0.69, p<0.05), in the
parietal gray matter (r = 0.91, p<0.05), in the temporal gray matter (r = 0.89, p<0.05) and in
the occipital gray matter (r = 0.88, p<0.05). The intradialytic weight change (3B) during the dialysis cycle was negatively correlated with parietal white matter water content (r = -0.74,
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
8 / 18
Quantitative Cerebral Water Mapping in Hemodialysis
Fig 1. Quantitative brain water content maps. Axial slices of water content maps obtained from a representative HD patient (a) and from an age-matched
healthy control (b). Differences between HD patients and healthy controls groups were assessed using non-parametric Wilcoxon rank-sum test and are
shown as an overlaid p-value map on a MNI template. Analyses of water content revealed increase of water content in predominantly white matter in HD
patients compared to controls. Hereby, enhanced brain hydration was found in particular in the parietal cortex, followed by occipital and fronto-temporal
regions (c). The color bar in a) and b) represents the water content in percent ranging from zero to hundred percent, the color bar in c) displays the p-values.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188.g001
p<0.01), in particular the precuneus (r = -0.71, p<0.05), occipital white matter (r = -0.68,
p<0.05) and the body of the corpus callosum (r = -0.72, p<0.05). Furthermore, intradialytic
weight change was negatively associated with parietal gray matter (r = -0.861, p<0.05), temporal gray matter (r = -0.71, p<0.05) and occipital gray mater water content (r = -0.81, p<0.01).
Intrinsic alertness was positively correlated with frontal white matter water content (r = 0.73,
p<0.05). We also found negative moderate to good correlation coefficients between the score
on the anxiety and the depression scale with global white matter (r = -0.74, p<0.05), parietal
white matter (r = -0.72, p<0.05) and temporal white matter water content (r = -0.78, p<0.05)
as well as the water content observed in the corpus callosum (r = -0.69, p<0.05) and in the precuneus (r = -0.74, p<0.05). Remaining associations were not significant.
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
9 / 18
Quantitative Cerebral Water Mapping in Hemodialysis
Fig 2. Histogram. Distributions of the brain water content (X-axis) and T1 (Y-axis) from HD patients (red thin histogram lines before dialysis, thin green
histograms lines after dialysis) and healthy controls (blue thin histogram lines for first time point, black for second time point) are shown. Left: Individual data
and mean value histograms (bold) for T1 (Y-axis, left) reveal no significant differences in HD patients compared to healthy controls. Lower panel: Individual
data as well as mean value histograms (bold) for water content (X-axis) reveal increased white matter water content (~2%) as shown by the observed shift,
but only mild changes in gray matter in HD patients compared to controls.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188.g002
Discussion
This is the first in vivo quantitative study based on quantitative water content estimation revealing evidence for abnormal brain water content mainly in white matter areas of cerebrum in
HD patients compared to healthy controls, as most likely long-term consequences of hemodialysis. Based on current results, such changes on water content are likely to be accounted for
chronic or long-term disease factors, such as dialysis vintage, and less influenced by variations
during the dialysis cycle, as inferred by the lack of relevant interaction effects in our data. Although few regions showed significant interactions, with a tendency of decreased water content
in HD patients after dialysis, whereas controls showed a marginal increase at re-test, similar to
previous studies [55], they might be likely related to method noise or error. Furthermore, one
of the major cognitive domains, altered in HD patients, namely attentional deficits, could be
linked to the increased frontal white matter water content.
Water in the human brain is located in distinctive microenvironments such as axons, neurons, glial cells, myelin sheaths, extracellular space, and blood vessels. The water content measurements for gray and white matter in our control group are in very good agreement with
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
10 / 18
Quantitative Cerebral Water Mapping in Hemodialysis
Table 3. Estimated water content vales in the brain of hemodialysis patients and healthy controls on t1 and t2 time-points.
Anatomical region
Hemodialysis Patients
(n = 10)
Healthy Controls (n = 10)
t1
Mean (SD)
t2
Mean (SD)
t1
Mean (SD)
t2
Mean (SD)
Mixed model
White matter
72.89 (1.61)
73.48 (2.47)
71.46 (1.06)
71.67 (1.21)
g*
Gray matter
84.42 (1.59)
84.15 (1.92)
82.93 (1.00)
83.62 (1.16)
ns
Frontal lobe
72.31 (1.68)
72.98 (2.40)
70.89 (1.24)
71.14 (1.36)
g*
Temporal lobe
72.23 (1.85)
72.96 (2.54)
70.52 (1.11)
70.71 (1.19)
g*
Parietal lobe
72.36 (1.65)
72.90 (2.69)
70.97 (0.91)
71.04 (1.24)
g*
Occipital lobe
73.06 (1.91)
73.64 (2.77)
71.59 (1.17)
71.76 (1.24)
g*
Cerebellum
74.95 (1.19)
75.28 (2.41)
73.91 (0.82)
74.13 (1.09)
ns
Frontal lobe
84.03 (1.38)
83.61 (2.07)
82.52 (1.00)
83.22 (1.39)
ns
Temporal lobe
84.14 (1.85)
84.02 (2.00)
82.38 (1.08)
82.91 (1.07)
g*
Parietal lobe
84.96 (2.27)
84.69 (2.27)
83.14 (1.31)
83.91 (1.18)
ns
Occipital lobe
84.54 (1.58)
84.05 (1.93)
83.08 (0.93)
83.89 (1.11)
i*
Cerebellum
84.77 (1.55)
84.86 (2.06)
83.73 (1.22)
84.29 (1.29)
ns
72.26 (2.15)
73.14 (2.82)
70.60 (1.11)
70.89 (1.22)
g*
83.54 (1.51)
82.88 (1.89)
81.75 (1.09)
82.59 (1.47)
i*
Global water content
White matter
Gray matter
Significant Subregions
Corpus callosum (body)
Frontal lobe
Inferior frontal gyrus pars triangularis (GM)
Inferior frontal gyrus pars opercularis (WM)
72.60 (1.50)
72.86 (2.30)
71.20 (1.65)
71.10 (1.40)
g*
Frontal pole (GM)
82.79 (1.46)
82.37 (1.98)
81.47 (1.08)
82.35 (1.50)
i*
Frontal operculum cortex (WM)
72.84 (1.68)
73.39 (2.69)
70.98 (1.41)
71.40 (1.61)
g**
Superior temporal gyrus posterior division (WM)
72.73 (1.67)
73.60 (3.14)
71.30 (1.60)
71.57 (1.52)
g*
Superior temporal gyrus anterior division (GM)
84.09 (2.19)
83.67 (2.25)
81.49 (1.44)
82.87 (1.49)
g*, i*
Superior temporal gyrus posterior division (GM)
84.67 (1.49)
84.01 (1.84)
82.22 (1.70)
83.35 (1.42)
g*, i*
Middle temporal gyrus temporooccipital part (WM)
73.41 (1.53)
73.94 (2.31)
71.69 (1.09)
71.91 (1.32)
g*
Temporal lobe
Inferior temporal gyrus anterior division (WM)
71.70 (2.08)
72.61 (2.67)
70.73 (1.11)
71.93 (0.97)
t**
Temporal pole (GM)
82.87 (1.39)
82.29 (2.01)
81.92 (1.18)
82.82 (1.34)
i*
Temporal fusiform cortex posterior division (GM)
84.96 (1.81)
84.44 (2.00)
82.90 (1.05)
83.76 (1.04)
g*
Hippocampus
85.83 (1.70)
85.42 (2.50)
83.79 (1.00)
84.50 (1.10)
g*
Parahippocampal gyrus anterior division (GM)
84.26 (2.07)
82.85 (2.21)
82.79 (1.49)
83.46 (1.35)
i*
Parahippocampal gyrus posterior division (WM)
74.98 (1.78)
75.41 (2.92)
73.15 (1.93)
72.93 (1.62)
g*
Central opercular cortex (WM)
73.02 (1.71)
73.63 (2.77)
71.02 (1.19)
71.18 (1.36)
g**
Central opercular cortex (GM)
84.52 (1.59)
84.47 (2.16)
82.50 (1.08)
83.20 (1.12)
g*
Insular cortex (WM)
73.26 (1.92)
74.14 (2.92)
71.89 (1.37)
71.99 (1.30)
g*
Postcentral gyrus (WM)
71.74 (1.57)
72.25 (2.38)
70.42 (1.32)
70.54 (1.36)
g*
Supramarginal gyrus anterior division (WM)
72.55 (1.94)
73.13 (2.45)
71.05 (1.49)
71.03 (1.46)
g*
Supramarginal gyrus anterior division (GM)
83.32 (1.75)
83.20 (1.73)
81.69 (1.08)
82.15 (1.16)
g*
Supramarginal gyrus posterior division (WM)
72.29 (1.68)
73.19 (2.21)
70.75 (1.33)
70.87 (1.22)
g*
Supramarginal gyrus posterior division (GM)
83.88 (1.78)
83.74 (1.72)
82.01 (1.20)
82.60 (0.99)
g*
Precuneous cortex (WM)
72.52 (1.60)
73.50 (3.14)
71.24 (1.38)
71.19 (1.35)
g*
Angular gyrus (WM)
72.30 (1.59)
73.05 (2.30)
70.49 (1.08)
70.74 (1.22)
g**
Parietal lobe
(Continued)
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
11 / 18
Quantitative Cerebral Water Mapping in Hemodialysis
Table 3. (Continued)
Anatomical region
Hemodialysis Patients
(n = 10)
Healthy Controls (n = 10)
t1
Mean (SD)
t2
Mean (SD)
t1
Mean (SD)
t2
Mean (SD)
Mixed model
Angular gyrus (GM)
84.12 (1.74)
84.07 (1.87)
82.16 (1.07)
82.69 (0.96)
g*
Parietal operculum cortex (GM)
84.33 (1.92)
84.60 (1.97)
82.42 (0.90)
83.16 (1.04)
g*
Intracalcarine cortex (WM)
72.84 (1.81)
73.60 (2.86)
71.76 (1.42)
71.44 (1.50)
g*
Supracalcarine cortex (GM)
84.61 (1.55)
84.54 (2.28)
82.83 (1.45)
83.42 (1.26)
g*
Lateral occipital cortex superior division (WM)
72.00 (1.46)
72.50 (2.36)
70.21 (1.13)
70.35 (1.45)
g*
Lateral occipital cortex superior division (GM)
83.55 (1.68)
83.50 (1.95)
81.62 (1.20)
82.13 (1.12)
g*
Lateral occipital cortex inferior division (WM)
72.68 (1.68)
72.97 (2.73)
71.02 (0.83)
71.24 (1.30)
g*
Occipital pole (GM)
84.09 (2.13)
83.90 (1.86)
82.16 (1.46)
82.97 (1.43)
g*
Occipital lobe
Note. Mean and standard deviation values from patient and control groups, comparison of the two time points (before dialysis t1 vs. after dialysis t2). Data
are given in percentage of pure water molarity; WM = white matter; GM = gray matter, remaining subregions were not significant. ns, not significant; i,
significant interaction of group and time; g, significant main effect of group; t significant effect of time. Mixed linear model Group x Time.
* p <. 05
** p <. 01.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188.t003
values from available neuroimaging literature (at 1.5T and 3T), including results from invasive
water content measurements in biopsy samples [14–16, 34, 36, 38, 56–59]. Changes in brain
water content and/or its spatial distribution, due to cerebral or extra-cerebral mediators, can
occur in both reversible and irreversible pathological conditions. Although we also revealed alterations of brain water content in the gray matter, quantitative water content maps results
mainly suggest a greater vulnerability of the white matter water content in HD patients. Similarly and to greater extend, patients with hepatic encephalopathy showed an increase in cerebral white matter water content whereas water content in gray matter was globally unaffected
[14], indicating an increased susceptibility of this neural tissue. Nevertheless, our results show
an abnormal accumulation of excess fluid leading to early stage cerebral edema. As any cell,
glia, axon and myelin sheath can be the target of toxic substances and reactive astrocytes play a
significant role in cellular and tissue repair by detoxifying various noxious substances, excitotoxic aspects leading to an (cytotoxic) edema may be involved. However, the increased uptake
of fluid results in local swelling, may compress the distinct microenvironments, which may finally result in dysfunctional states; and accompany various processes that damage cells, even in
the early phase of degeneration. Consequently, injured cells (neurons) can shrink and disintegrate, with time degeneration, and gliosis can develop. In this context, it has to be noted that—
although several neuroimaging studies in HD patients reported predominantly white matter atrophy, and to a lesser extent also in gray matter [60–66]—our sample showed only marginal
changes of reduced white matter volume.
However, being largest white matter fiber bundle in the human brain, containing more than
300 million axons, the corpus callosum showed an increase of brain water content in HD patients, which was associated with longer duration of dialysis vintage und lower intradialytic
weight change. Due to its highly interwoven connections the corpus callosum is integral in relaying sensory, motor, cognitive and emotional information between homologous regions in
the cerebral hemispheres. Consequently, abnormalities by water diffusion in the corpus callosum may result in a broad variety of dysfunctions. The observed association between anxiety
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
12 / 18
Quantitative Cerebral Water Mapping in Hemodialysis
Fig 3. Association between brain water content and clinical parameters. Boxplots illustrating the association between water content in different focal
subregions and clinical parameters. Fig 3A suggests that increased water content in several white and gray matter regions is associated with longer dialysis
vintage. In Fig 3B the association between smaller intradialytic weight changes and increased water content in white and grey matter structures is shown.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188.g003
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
13 / 18
Quantitative Cerebral Water Mapping in Hemodialysis
and depression related to an increase in the corpus callosum water content corroborates the
previous findings of emotional dysregulation based on white matter abnormalities of the corpus callosum affecting the limbic-cortical network [67].
Major areas of hydration alterations in HD compared to controls were also found in the parietal white matter. These could be in part linked to clinical variables. Our results are in accordance with previous studies evoking the association between dialysis vintage and the presence
of cognitive impairment, and white matter abnormalities [64], which reinforces the idea that
longer dialysis vintage may add to an already increased vulnerability to repeated brain injury
and cognitive impairment [68], through several potential pathomechanisms. One of the factors
potentially involved is dialysis-related hypotension and associated changes in brain perfusion
and metabolism [69]. A study that investigated the role of intradialytic hypotension in brain atrophy in chronic HD patients, established a positive relation between the incidence of hypotensive episodes during dialysis and the number of lacunae, but mainly focussed on the
association between hypotension and the progression of frontal lobe atrophy over three years
[62]. Although we could not establish a direct link to brain water content measures and damage
to fronto-subcortical areas, which are supposed to have an increased sensitivity to cerebrovascular damage [69, 70], we hypothesize that the importance of frontal areas may be further emphasized by the link between water content in frontal areas and cognitive performance,
accentuating the significance of the frontal-attentional/executive domain in the cognitive dysfunction profile of patients undergoing hemodialysis [71–73]. This neuropsychological profile
of attentional and executive dysfunction was again observed in our sample, as we already described in a larger sample [19].
Furthermore, alterations in the posteromedial portion of the parietal lobe and precuneus
resonate well with recent findings, that this region plays an essential role in multiple higherorder cognitive processes [74]. Moreover, they are included in the brain structures with the
highest resting metabolic rates, and constitute a major part in the default mode network, which
is a high level or organized default functional activity at rest [75], and which can pinpoint distinct disease-specific alterations, such as in Alzheimer’s disease [76].
Also in line with previous studies is the observation of the association between anxiety and
depression and white matter, as well gray matter changes [77, 78], which might be mirrored
here with the increase of water content in white matter of the temporo-parietal cortex and the
corpus callosum.
This is the first in vivo study in HD investigating brain water content. We can state that
water homeostasis is altered in HD patients, most likely as chronic effect of hemodialysis, not
excluding the role of other factors. However, the results of this study and its interpretations are
limited due to the selected small-sized sample. Nevertheless, it reinforces the necessity to take
physiological values, as well as complementary metabolic imaging methods into account when
investigating brain alterations in this population.
Acknowledgments
We sincerely thank all our patients and control subjects for their enduring collaboration and
interest in this research.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: KR NJS JBS ZA AC. Performed the experiments: ZA
AC SM KR. Analyzed the data: ZA AC KR FTR. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools:
ZA VG SM JF BH RDF AV TK FE TG KR. Wrote the paper: KR ZA AC. Revision of manuscript: KR ZA AC VG FTR SM BH RDF AV TK FE TG JBS JF NJS.
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
14 / 18
Quantitative Cerebral Water Mapping in Hemodialysis
References
1.
Sanchez-Roman S, Ostrosky-Solis F, Morales-Buenrostro LE, Nogues-Vizcaino MG, Alberu J, McClintock SM. Neurocognitive profile of an adult sample with chronic kidney disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS. 2011; 17(1):80–90. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710001219
PMID: 21062523.
2.
Yaffe K, Ackerson L, Kurella Tamura M, Le Blanc P, Kusek JW, Sehgal AR, et al. Chronic kidney disease and cognitive function in older adults: findings from the chronic renal insufficiency cohort cognitive
study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2010; 58(2):338–45. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2009.
02670.x PMID: 20374407; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2852884.
3.
Graham JE, Rockwood K, Beattie BL, Eastwood R, Gauthier S, Tuokko H, et al. Prevalence and severity of cognitive impairment with and without dementia in an elderly population. Lancet. 1997; 349
(9068):1793–6. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)01007-6 PMID: 9269213.
4.
Murray AM, Tupper DE, Knopman DS, Gilbertson DT, Pederson SL, Li S, et al. Cognitive impairment in
hemodialysis patients is common. Neurology. 2006; 67(2):216–23. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000225182.
15532.40 PMID: 16864811.
5.
Mogi M, Horiuchi M. Clinical Interaction between Brain and Kidney in Small Vessel Disease. Cardiology
research and practice. 2011; 2011:306189. doi: 10.4061/2011/306189 PMID: 21274446; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3025374.
6.
Ikram MA, Vernooij MW, Hofman A, Niessen WJ, van der Lugt A, Breteler MM. Kidney function is related to cerebral small vessel disease. Stroke; a journal of cerebral circulation. 2008; 39(1):55–61. doi: 10.
1161/STROKEAHA.107.493494 PMID: 18048865.
7.
Kobayashi M, Hirawa N, Yatsu K, Kobayashi Y, Yamamoto Y, Saka S, et al. Relationship between silent brain infarction and chronic kidney disease. Nephrology, dialysis, transplantation: official publication of the European Dialysis and Transplant Association—European Renal Association. 2009; 24
(1):201–7. doi: 10.1093/ndt/gfn419 PMID: 18697797; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2639313.
8.
Murray AM. Cognitive impairment in the aging dialysis and chronic kidney disease populations: an occult burden. Advances in chronic kidney disease. 2008; 15(2):123–32. doi: 10.1053/j.ackd.2008.01.010
PMID: 18334236; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2504691.
9.
Bugnicourt JM, Godefroy O, Chillon JM, Choukroun G, Massy ZA. Cognitive disorders and dementia
in CKD: the neglected kidney-brain axis. Journal of the American Society of Nephrology: JASN. 2013;
24(3):353–63. doi: 10.1681/ASN.2012050536 PMID: 23291474.
10.
O'Brien JT, Erkinjuntti T, Reisberg B, Roman G, Sawada T, Pantoni L, et al. Vascular cognitive impairment. Lancet neurology. 2003; 2(2):89–98. PMID: 12849265.
11.
Lux S, Mirzazade S, Kuzmanovic B, Plewan T, Eickhoff SB, Shah NJ, et al. Differential activation of
memory-relevant brain regions during a dialysis cycle. Kidney international. 2010; 78(8):794–802. doi:
10.1038/ki.2010.253 PMID: 20668428.
12.
Murray AM, Pederson SL, Tupper DE, Hochhalter AK, Miller WA, Li Q, et al. Acute variation in cognitive
function in hemodialysis patients: a cohort study with repeated measures. American journal of kidney
diseases: the official journal of the National Kidney Foundation. 2007; 50(2):270–8. doi: 10.1053/j.ajkd.
2007.05.010 PMID: 17660028.
13.
Pereira AA, Weiner DE, Scott T, Sarnak MJ. Cognitive function in dialysis patients. American journal of
kidney diseases: the official journal of the National Kidney Foundation. 2005; 45(3):448–62. doi: 10.
1053/j.ajkd.2004.10.024 PMID: 15754267.
14.
Shah NJ, Neeb H, Kircheis G, Engels P, Haussinger D, Zilles K. Quantitative cerebral water content
mapping in hepatic encephalopathy. NeuroImage. 2008; 41(3):706–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.
2008.02.057 PMID: 18456518.
15.
Abbas Z, Gras V, Mollenhoff K, Oros-Peusquens AM, Shah NJ. Quantitative water content mapping at
clinically relevant field strengths: A comparative study at 1.5T and 3T. NeuroImage. 2015; 106:404–13.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.017 PMID: 25463455.
16.
Abbas Z, Gras V, Mollenhoff K, Keil F, Oros-Peusquens AM, Shah NJ. Analysis of proton-density bias
corrections based on T measurement for robust quantification of water content in the brain at 3 Tesla.
Magnetic resonance in medicine: official journal of the Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine / Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2014. doi: 10.1002/mrm.25086 PMID: 24436248.
17.
Herrmann-Lingen C, Buss U, Snaith RP. Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale—deutsche Version
(HADS-D) Bern: Verlag Hans Huber; 1995.
18.
Radbruch L, Sabatowski R, Elsner F, Everts J, Mendoza T, Cleeland C. Validation of the German Version of the Brief Fatigue Inventory. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management. 2003; 25(5):449–58.
doi: 10.1016/S0885-3924(03)00073-3 PMID: 12727043
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
15 / 18
Quantitative Cerebral Water Mapping in Hemodialysis
19.
Costa AS, Tiffin-Richards FE, Holschbach B, Frank RD, Vassiliadou A, Kruger T, et al. Clinical Predictors of Individual Cognitive Fluctuations in Patients Undergoing Hemodialysis. American journal of kidney diseases: the official journal of the National Kidney Foundation. 2014. doi: 10.1053/j.ajkd.2014.02.
012 PMID: 24679895.
20.
Kessler J, Markowitsch HJ, Denzler P. Mini-Mental-Status-Test (MMST) [German Adaptation]. Göttingen: Beltz Test GMBH; 2000.
21.
Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The Montreal
Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2005; 53(4):695–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x PMID: 15817019
22.
Costa AS, Fimm B, Friesen P, Soundjock H, Rottschy C, Gross T, et al. Alternate-Form Reliability of the
Montreal Cognitive Assessment Screening Test in a Clinical Setting. Dementia and geriatric cognitive
disorders. 2012; 33(6):379–84. doi: 10.1159/000340006 PMID: 22797211
23.
Zimmermann P, Fimm B. Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung. Version 2.3. Herzogenrath: Psytest; 2012.
24.
Wagner S, Helmreich I, Dahmen N, Lieb K, Tadic A. Reliability of three alternate forms of the trail making tests a and B. Archives of clinical neuropsychology: the official journal of the National Academy of
Neuropsychologists. 2011; 26(4):314–21. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acr024
25.
von Aste M, Neubauer A, Horn R. Wechsler Intelligenztest für Erwachsene (WIE). Deutschsprachige
Bearbeitung und Adaptation des WAIS-III von David Wechsler.. Frankfurt: Harcourt Test Services;
2006.
26.
Aschenbrenner S, Tucha O, Lange KW. Regensburg Word Fluency Test [Regensburger Wortflüssigkeits-Test (RWT)]. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2000.
27.
Bäumler G. Farbe-Wort-Interferenztest. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1985.
28.
Niemann H, Sturm W, Thöne-Otto A, Willmes K. California Verbal Learning Test—German adaptation.
Frankfurt: Pearson Assessment; 2008.
29.
Aebi C. Valiedirung der neuropsychologischen Testbatterie CERAD-NP: eine Multi-Center Studie
2002.
30.
Warrington E, James M. Testbatterie für Visuelle Objekt- und Raumwahrnehmung (VOSP). Frankfurt:
Harcourt Test Services; 1992.
31.
Beddhu S, Bruns FJ, Saul M, Seddon P, Zeidel ML. A simple comorbidity scale predicts clinical outcomes and costs in dialysis patients. The American journal of medicine. 2000; 108(8):609–13. PMID:
10856407.
32.
Charlson ME, Pompei P, Ales KL, MacKenzie CR. A new method of classifying prognostic comorbidity
in longitudinal studies: development and validation. Journal of chronic diseases. 1987; 40(5):373–83.
PMID: 3558716.
33.
Conroy RM, Pyorala K, Fitzgerald AP, Sans S, Menotti A, De Backer G, et al. Estimation of ten-year risk
of fatal cardiovascular disease in Europe: the SCORE project. European heart journal. 2003; 24(11):
987–1003. PMID: 12788299.
34.
Neeb H, Ermer V, Stocker T, Shah NJ. Fast quantitative mapping of absolute water content with full
brain coverage. NeuroImage. 2008; 42(3):1094–109. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.060 PMID:
18632287.
35.
Neeb H, Shah NJ. Enhancing the precision of quantitative water content mapping by optimizing sequence parameters. Magnetic resonance in medicine: official journal of the Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine / Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2006; 56(1):224–9. doi: 10.1002/mrm.
20929 PMID: 16732595.
36.
Neeb H, Zilles K, Shah NJ. A new method for fast quantitative mapping of absolute water content in
vivo. NeuroImage. 2006; 31(3):1156–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.063 PMID: 16650780.
37.
Neeb H, Zilles K, Shah NJ. Fully-automated detection of cerebral water content changes: study of ageand gender-related H2O patterns with quantitative MRI. NeuroImage. 2006; 29(3):910–22. doi: 10.
1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.062 PMID: 16303316.
38.
Shah NJ, Ermer V, Oros-Peusquens AM. Measuring the absolute water content of the brain using
quantitative MRI. Methods in molecular biology. 2011; 711:29–64. doi: 10.1007/978-1-61737-992-5_3
PMID: 21279597.
39.
Shah NJ, Neeb H, Zaitsev M, Steinhoff S, Kircheis G, Amunts K, et al. Quantitative T1 mapping of hepatic encephalopathy using magnetic resonance imaging. Hepatology. 2003; 38(5):1219–26. doi: 10.
1053/jhep.2003.50477 PMID: 14578860.
40.
Gras V, Abbas Z, Shah NJ. Spoiled FLASH MRI with slice selective excitation: Signal equation with a
correction term. Concepts in Magnetic Resonance Part A. 2013; 42(3):89–100. doi: 10.1002/cmr.a.
21264
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
16 / 18
Quantitative Cerebral Water Mapping in Hemodialysis
41.
Tofts PS. Quantitative MRI of the Brain. PD: proton density of tissue water. NJ: Wiley & Sons Ltd.
2003:p:85–109.
42.
Griswold MA, Blaimer M, Breuer F, Heidemann RM, Mueller M, Jakob PM. Parallel magnetic resonance
imaging using the GRAPPA operator formalism. Magnetic resonance in medicine: official journal of the
Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine / Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2005; 54(6):
1553–6. doi: 10.1002/mrm.20722 PMID: 16254956.
43.
Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Voxel-based morphometry—the methods. NeuroImage. 2000; 11(6 Pt 1):
805–21. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0582 PMID: 10860804.
44.
Jenkinson M, Smith S. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Medical image analysis. 2001; 5(2):143–56. PMID: 11516708.
45.
Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Unified segmentation. NeuroImage. 2005; 26(3):839–51. doi: 10.1016/j.
neuroimage.2005.02.018 PMID: 15955494.
46.
Keil F, Oros-Peusquens AM, Shah NJ. Investigation of the spatial correlation in human white matter
and the influence of age using 3-dimensional variography applied to MP-RAGE data. NeuroImage.
2012; 63(3):1374–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.07.034 PMID: 22836175.
47.
Biernacki C, Celeux G, Govaert G, Langrognet F. Model-based cluster and discriminant analysis with
the MIXMOD software. Computational Statistics & Data Analysis. 2006; 51(2):587–600. doi: 10.1016/j.
csda.2005.12.015
48.
Collins DL, Holmes CJ, Peters TM, Evans AC. Automatic 3-D model-based neuroanatomical segmentation. Human Brain Mapping. 1995; 3(3):190–208. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460030304
49.
Makris N, Goldstein JM, Kennedy D, Hodge SM, Caviness VS, Faraone SV, et al. Decreased volume of
left and total anterior insular lobule in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia research. 2006; 83(2–3):155–71.
doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.11.020 PMID: 16448806.
50.
Goldstein JM, Seidman LJ, Makris N, Ahern T, O'Brien LM, Caviness VS Jr., et al. Hypothalamic abnormalities in schizophrenia: sex effects and genetic vulnerability. Biological psychiatry. 2007; 61(8):
935–45. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.027 PMID: 17046727.
51.
Frazier JA, Chiu S, Breeze JL, Makris N, Lange N, Kennedy DN, et al. Structural brain magnetic resonance imaging of limbic and thalamic volumes in pediatric bipolar disorder. The American journal of
psychiatry. 2005; 162(7):1256–65. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.7.1256 PMID: 15994707.
52.
Desikan RS, Segonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, et al. An automated labeling
system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest.
NeuroImage. 2006; 31(3):968–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021 PMID: 16530430.
53.
Jahng G-H, Stables L, Ebel A, Matson GB, Meyerhoff DJ, Weiner MW, et al. Sensitive and fast T[sub 1]
mapping based on two inversion recovery images and a reference image. Medical Physics. 2005; 32
(6):1524. doi: 10.1118/1.1915014 PMID: 16013709
54.
Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1988.
55.
Walters G, Habib AM, Reynolds J, Wu H, Knight JF, Pusey CD. Glomerular T cells are of restricted
clonality and express multiple CDR3 motifs across different Vbeta T-cell receptor families in experimental autoimmune glomerulonephritis. Nephron Experimental nephrology. 2004; 98(3):e71–81. doi: 10.
1159/000080682 PMID: 15528947.
56.
Fatouros PP, Marmarou A. Use of magnetic resonance imaging for in vivo measurements of water content in human brain: method and normal values. Journal of neurosurgery. 1999; 90(1):109–15. doi: 10.
3171/jns.1999.90.1.0109 PMID: 10413163.
57.
Oros-Peusquens AM KF, Abbas N, Gras V, Möllenhof K, Shah NJ. A 7 min protocol for quantitative,
whole-brain, accurate water mapping at 3 T for neurological applications. Proc Intl Soc Magn Reson
Med. 2012; 20.
58.
Warntjes JB, Dahlqvist O, Lundberg P. Novel method for rapid, simultaneous T1, T*2, and proton density quantification. Magnetic resonance in medicine: official journal of the Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine / Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2007; 57(3):528–37. doi: 10.1002/
mrm.21165 PMID: 17326183.
59.
Whittall KP, MacKay AL, Graeb DA, Nugent RA, Li DK, Paty DW. In vivo measurement of T2 distributions and water contents in normal human brain. Magnetic resonance in medicine: official journal of the
Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine / Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 1997; 37(1):
34–43. PMID: 8978630.
60.
Yakushiji Y, Nanri Y, Hirotsu T, Nishihara M, Hara M, Nakajima J, et al. Marked cerebral atrophy is correlated with kidney dysfunction in nondisabled adults. Hypertension research: official journal of the Japanese Society of Hypertension. 2010; 33(12):1232–7. doi: 10.1038/hr.2010.171 PMID: 20944639.
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
17 / 18
Quantitative Cerebral Water Mapping in Hemodialysis
61.
Kurt A, Firat A, Agildere AM, Aytekin C, Benli S, Özmen I, et al. Evaluation of MRI-detected White Matter Changes in Relation to Hemodialysis Duration and Renal Transplantation. Rivista di Neuroradiologia. 2003; 16:275–82.
62.
Mizumasa T, Hirakata H, Yoshimitsu T, Hirakata E, Kubo M, Kashiwagi M, et al. Dialysis-related hypotension as a cause of progressive frontal lobe atrophy in chronic hemodialysis patients: a 3-year prospective study. Nephron Clinical practice. 2004; 97(1):c23–30. doi: 10.1159/000077592 PMID:
15153764.
63.
Savazzi GM, Cusmano F, Musini S. Cerebral imaging changes in patients with chronic renal failure
treated conservatively or in hemodialysis. Nephron. 2001; 89(1):31–6. doi: 46040 PMID: 11528229.
64.
Hsieh TJ, Chang JM, Chuang HY, Ko CH, Hsieh ML, Liu GC, et al. End-stage renal disease: in vivo diffusion-tensor imaging of silent white matter damage. Radiology. 2009; 252(2):518–25. doi: 10.1148/
radiol.2523080484 PMID: 19528357.
65.
Drew DA, Bhadelia R, Tighiouart H, Novak V, Scott TM, Lou KV, et al. Anatomic brain disease in hemodialysis patients: a cross-sectional study. American journal of kidney diseases: the official journal of the
National Kidney Foundation. 2013; 61(2):271–8. doi: 10.1053/j.ajkd.2012.08.035 PMID: 23040011;
PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3546146.
66.
Kong X, Wen JQ, Qi RF, Luo S, Zhong JH, Chen HJ, et al. Diffuse interstitial brain edema in patients
with end-stage renal disease undergoing hemodialysis: a tract-based spatial statistics study. Medicine.
2014; 93(28):e313. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000000313 PMID: 25526483.
67.
Kieseppä T, Eerola M, Mäntylä R, Neuvonen T, Poutanen V-P, Luoma K, et al. Major depressive disorder and white matter abnormalities: A diffusion tensor imaging study with tract-based spatial statistics.
Journal of Affective Disorders. 2010; 120(1–3):240–4. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.04.023 PMID: 19735945
68.
Williams MA, Sklar AH, Burright RG, Donovick PJ. Temporal effects of dialysis on cognitive functioning
in patients with ESRD. American journal of kidney diseases. 2004; 43(4):705–11. PMID: 15042548
69.
Eldehni MT, McIntyre CW. Are there neurological consequences of recurrent intradialytic hypotension?
Seminars in dialysis. 2012; 25(3):253–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-139X.2012.01057.x PMID: 22353138
70.
Madero M, Sarnak MJ. Does hemodialysis hurt the brain? Seminars in dialysis. 2011; 24(3):266–8. doi:
10.1111/j.1525-139X.2011.00857.x PMID: 21435001
71.
Pereira AA, Weiner DE, Scott T, Chandra P, Bluestein R, Griffith J, et al. Subcortical cognitive impairment in dialysis patients. Hemodialysis international. 2007; 11(3):309–14. doi: 10.1111/j.15424758.2007.00185.x PMID: 17576295
72.
Post JB, Jegede AB, Morin K, Spungen AM, Langhoff E, Sano M. Cognitive profile of chronic kidney
disease and hemodialysis patients without dementia. Nephron Clinical practice. 2010; 116(3):c247–55.
doi: 10.1159/000317206 PMID: 20606486
73.
Sarnak MJ, Tighiouart H, Scott TM, Lou KV, Sorensen EP, Giang LM, et al. Frequency of and risk factors for poor cognitive performance in hemodialysis patients. Neurology. 2013; 80(5):471–80. doi: 10.
1212/WNL.0b013e31827f0f7f PMID: 23303848
74.
Cavanna AE, Trimble MR. The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain: a journal of neurology. 2006; 129(Pt 3):564–83. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl004 PMID:
16399806.
75.
Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain
function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2001;
98(2):676–82. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676 PMID: 11209064; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC14647.
76.
Greicius MD, Srivastava G, Reiss AL, Menon V. Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer's disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences of the United States of America. 2004; 101(13):4637–42. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308627101
PMID: 15070770; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC384799.
77.
Disabato BM, Sheline YI. Biological basis of late life depression. Current psychiatry reports. 2012;
14(4):273–9. doi: 10.1007/s11920-012-0279-6 PMID: 22562412; PubMed Central PMCID:
PMC3752388.
78.
Herrmann LL, Le Masurier M, Ebmeier KP. White matter hyperintensities in late life depression: a systematic review. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry. 2008; 79(6):619–24. doi: 10.1136/
jnnp.2007.124651 PMID: 17717021.
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0122188 March 31, 2015
18 / 18