Dyslexia: Difference between revisions
Undid revision 300589819 by 86.130.167.14 (talk) |
Cityzen451 (talk | contribs) |
||
Line 779: | Line 779: | ||
* [[Child development]] |
* [[Child development]] |
||
* [[Dyslexia in fiction]] |
* [[Dyslexia in fiction]] |
||
==Further Reading== |
|||
{{cite book |title=The Gift of Dyslexia: Why Some of the Brightest People Can't Read and How They Can Learn |last=Davis |first=Ronald |year=1997 |publisher=Souvenir Press Ltd |location= |isbn=0285634127 |pages=272 |url=http://www.amazon.co.uk/Gift-Dyslexia-Brightest-People-Learn/dp/0285634127/ref=sr_1_1?ie=UTF8&s=books&qid=1246885121&sr=8-1 }} |
|||
{{cite book |title=A Memory Therapy & Study Guide For Dyslexia, Adhd, Learning Impairment & Poor Verbal Memory: Improve Your Verbal Memory |last=Rowan |first=James |year=2008 |publisher=Createspace Ltd |location= |isbn=1440495238 |pages= |url=http://www.amazon.com/Memory-Therapy-Dyslexia-Learning-Impairment/dp/1440495238/ref=sr_1_1?ie=UTF8&s=books&qid=1246880031&sr=8-1 }} |
|||
{{cite book |title=How to Identify and Support Children with Dyslexia |last=Neanon|first=Chris |year=2002 |publisher=LDA |location= |isbn=1855033569 |pages=64 |url=http://www.amazon.co.uk/How-Identify-Support-Children-Dyslexia/dp/1855033569/ref=sr_1_2?ie=UTF8&s=books&qid=1246885121&sr=8-2 }} |
|||
==References== |
==References== |
Revision as of 13:42, 6 July 2009
Dyslexia is a learning disability that makes itself manifest primarily as a difficulty with the visual notation of speech or written language, particularly with reading the various man-made writing systems. It is separate and distinct from reading difficulties resulting from other causes, such as a non-neurological deficiency with vision or hearing, or from poor or inadequate reading instruction.[1] This suggests that dyslexia results from differences in how the brain processes written and spoken language.
Definition
There are many definitions of dyslexia but no consensus. Some definitions are purely descriptive, while others embody causal theories. It appears that ‘dyslexia’ is not one thing but many, in so far as it serves as a conceptual clearing-house for a number of reading skills deficits and difficulties, with a number of causes.[2]
The majority of currently available dyslexia research relates to the alphabet writing system, and especially languages of European origin, however more research is becoming available regarding dyslexia and the other writing systems. This will provide a broader and more universal perspective of dyslexia.
Castles and Coltheart, 1993, described phonological and surface types of developmental dyslexia by analogy to classical subtypes of acquired dyslexia (alexia) which are classified according to the rate of errors in reading non-words.[3] However the distinction between surface and phonlogical dyslexia has not replaced the old empirical terminology of dysphonetic versus dyseidetic types of dyslexia (Boder 1973). The surface/phonological distinction is only descriptive, and devoid of any aetiological assumption as to the underlying brain mechanisms, where as the dysphonetic/dyseidetic distinction refers to two different mechanisms one relates to a speech discrimination deficit, and the other to a visual preception impairment. (Most people with dyslexia who have Boder's Dysiedetic type, have attentional and spatial difficulties which interfere with the reading acquisition process.[4]
Although dyslexia is thought to be the result of a neurological difference, it is not an intellectual disability. Dyslexia is diagnosed in people of all levels of intelligence.[5][6]
Diagnosis
Formal diagnosis of dyslexia is made by a qualified professional, such as a neurologist or an educational psychologist. Evaluation generally includes testing of reading ability together with measures of underlying skills such as tests of rapid naming to evaluate short term memory and sequencing skills, and nonword reading to evaluate phonological coding skills. Evaluation will usually also include an IQ test to establish a profile of learning strengths and weaknesses. While such "discrepancy" tests between full scale IQ and reading level have, on their own, been shown to be flawed,[7], the tests often include interdisciplinary testing to exclude other possible causes for reading difficulties, such as a more generalized cognitive impairment or physical causes such as problems with vision or hearing.
Leppänen PH et al., investigated whether children born to families with a history of dyslexia are at an elevated risk for the disorder. They studied 6-month-old infants with and without a high risk of familial dyslexia and measured differences in electrical activation in the brain generated by changes in the temporal structure of speech sounds, a critical cuing feature in speech. The infants at risk differed from control infants in both their initial responsiveness to sounds and in their change-detection responses dependent on the stimulus context. This indicates that infants at risk due to a familial background of reading problems process auditory temporal cues of speech sounds differently than infants without such risk even before they learn to speak, and that incidence of familial dyslexia may assist in diagnosis.[8] [9]
Recent dyslexia research using neuroimaging suggests that it may one day be possible to identify children with dyslexia before they learn to read.[8] [9]
Disagreement exists in dyslexia research as to whether dyslexia exists as a condition, or whether it simply reflects individual differences among different readers.[10]
Variations and related conditions
Dyslexia has many underlying causes that are believed to be a neurological conditions that influence the ability to read written language.[4]
The following conditions may be contributory or overlapping factors, or underlying cause of the dyslexic symptoms as they can lead to difficulty reading:
- Auditory processing disorder is a condition that affects the ability to process auditory information. Auditory Processing Disorder is a Listening Disability.[11] It can lead to problems with auditory memory and auditory sequencing. Many people with dyslexia have auditory processing problems including history of auditory reversals, and may develop their own Logographic cues to compensate for this type of deficit. Auditory processing disorder is recognized as one of the major causes of dyslexia.[11][12][13][14] Some children can acquire auditory processing disorder as a result of experiencing otitis media with effusion (Glue Ear, Sticky Ear, Grommits) and other severe ear conditions.[citation needed]
- Scotopic sensitivity syndrome, also known as Irlen Syndrome, is a term used to describe sensitivity to certain wavelengths of light which interfere with visual processing.[15][16]
- Developmental dyspraxia is a neurological condition characterized by a marked difficulty in carrying out routine tasks involving balance, fine-motor control, kinesthetic coordination, difficulty in the use of speech sounds, problems with short term memory and organization are typical of dyspraxics.
The following are related variations with possible shared underlying neurological causes.[citation needed]
- Dysgraphia is a disorder which expresses itself primarily during writing or typing, although in some cases it may also affect eye-hand coordination in such direction or sequence oriented processes as tying knots or carrying out a repetitive task. Dysgraphia is distinct from Dyspraxia in that the person may have the word to be written or the proper order of steps in mind clearly, but carries the sequence out in the wrong order.
- Dyscalculia is a neurological condition characterized by a problem with learning fundamentals and one or more of the basic numerical skills. Often people with this condition can understand very complex mathematical concepts and principles but have difficulty processing formulas and even basic addition and subtraction.
- Specific Language Impairment is a developmental language disorder that can affect both expressive and receptive language. SLI is defined as a "pure" language impairment, meaning that is not related to or caused by other developmental disorders, hearing loss or acquired brain injury. A study by the Universities of Maastricht and Utrecht examined speech perception and speech production in 3-year-old Dutch children at familial risk of developing dyslexia. Their performance in speech sound categorisation and their production of words was compared to that of age-matched children with specific language impairment (SLI) and typically developing controls. The results of the at-risk and SLI-group were highly similar. Analysis of the individual data revealed that both groups contained subgroups with good and poorly performing children. Their impaired expressive phonology seemed to be related to a deficit in speech perception. The findings indicate that both dyslexia and SLI can be explained by a multi-risk model which includes cognitive processes as well as genetic factors.[19]
- Cluttering is a speech fluency disorder involving both the rate and rhythm of speech, and resulting in impaired speech intelligibility. Speech is erratic and dysrhythmic, consisting of rapid and jerky spurts that usually involve faulty phrasing. The personality of the clutterer bears striking resemblance to the personalities of those with learning disabilities.[20]
Characteristics
Some shared symptoms of the speech/hearing deficits and dyslexia:[citation needed]
- Confusion with before/after, right/left, and so on
- Difficulty learning the alphabet
- Difficulty with word retrieval or naming problems
- Difficulty identifying or generating rhyming words, or counting syllables in words (phonological awareness)
- Difficulty with hearing and manipulating sounds in words (phonemic awareness)
- Difficulty distinguishing different sounds in words (auditory discrimination)
- Difficulty in learning the sounds of letters
- Difficulty associating individual words with their correct meanings
- Difficulty with time keeping and concept of time
- Confusion with combinations of words
- Due to fear of speaking incorrectly, some children become withdrawn and shy or become bullies out of their inability to understand the social cues in their environment
- Difficulty in organization skills
The identification of these factors results from the study of patterns across many clinical observations of dyslexic children. In the UK, Thomas Richard Miles was important in such work and his observations led him to develop the Bangor Dyslexia Diagnostic Test.[21]
Dyslexia is about having problems with the visual notation of speech, which in most languages of European origin are problems with alphabet writing systems which have a phonetic construction. Experience of speech acquisition delays, and speech and language problems can be due to problems processing and decoding auditory input prior to reproducing their own version of speech, and may be observed as stuttering, cluttering or hesitant speech.[22][23]
These issues can be a shared component in the spelling and writing difficulties which people who have dyslexia can experience
Research into the Causes
- History
- Identified by Oswald Berkhan in 1881,[24] the term 'dyslexia' was later coined in 1887 by Rudolf Berlin,[25] an ophthalmologist practicing in Stuttgart, Germany.[26]
- In 1896, W. Pringle Morgan published a description of a reading-specific learning disorder in the British Medical Journal "Congenital Word Blindness".[27]
- During the 1890s and early 1900s, James Hinshelwood published a series of articles in medical journals describing similar cases of congenital word blindness. In his 1917 book Congenital Word Blindness, Hinshelwood asserted that the primary disability was in visual memory for words and letters, and described symptoms including letter reversals, and difficulties with spelling and reading comprehension.[28]
- 1925 Samuel T. Orton determined that there was a syndrome unrelated to brain damage that made learning to read difficult. Orton's theory strephosymbolia described individuals with dyslexia having difficulty associating the visual forms of words with their spoken forms.[29] Orton observed that reading deficits in dyslexia did not seem to stem from strictly visual deficits.[30] He believed the condition was caused by the failure to establish hemispheric dominance in the brain.[31] Orton later worked with psychologist and educator Anna Gillingham to develop an educational intervention that pioneered the use of simultaneous multisensory instruction.[32]
- In contrast, Dearborn, Gates, Bennet and Blau considered a faulty guidance of the seeing mechanism to be the cause. They sought to discover if a conflict between spontaneous orientation of the scanning action of the eyes from right to left and training aimed at the acquisition of an opposite direction would allow an interpretation of the facts observed in the dyslexic disorder and especially of the ability to mirror-read.
- 1949 research conducted under Clement Launay (thesis G. Mahec Paris 1951) went further. The phenomenon is clearly linked to the dynamics of sight as it disappears when the space between letters is increased, transforming the reading into spelling. This experience also explains the ability to mirror-read.
- In the 1970s, a new hypothesis emerged that dyslexia stems from a deficit in phonological processing or difficulty in recognizing that spoken words are formed by discrete phonemes affected individuals have difficulty associating these sounds with the visual letters that make up written words. Key studies suggested the importance of phonological awareness,[33]
- 1979 Galaburda and Kemper,[34] and Galaburda et al. 1985,[35] from the examination of post autopsy brains of people with dyslexia. Observed anatomical differences in the language center in a dyslexic brain, these studies and those of Cohen et al. 1989,[36] suggested abnormal cortical development which was presumed to occur before or during the sixth month of foetal brain development.[4]
- 1993 Castles and Coltheart describe developmental dyslexia as two prevalent and distinct varieties using the subtypes of Alexia, Surface and Phonological Dyslexia.[3] Manis et al. 1996, concluded that there were probably more than two subtypes of dyslexia, which would be related to multiple underlying deficits.[37]
- 1994 From post autopsy specimens Galaburda et al., reported : Abnormal auditory processing in people with dyslexia suggests that accompanying anatomical abnormalities might be present in the auditory system. Supported the reported behavioral findings of a left hemisphere-based phonological defect in dyslexic individuals.[38]
- The development of neuroimaging technologies during the 1980s and 1990s enabled dyslexia research to make significant advances. Positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have revealed the neural signature of adult normal reading (e.g. Fiez and Petersen, 1998;[39] Turkeltaub et al., 2002[40]) and phonological processing (e.g., Gelfand and Bookheimer, 2003;[41] Poldrack et al., 1999[42]). Employing various experimental approaches and paradigms (e.g., the detection or judgment of rhymes, nonword reading, and implicit reading), these studies have localized dysfunctional phonological processing in dyslexia to left-hemisphere perisylvian regions, especially for the alphabetic writing system (Paulesu et al., 2001;[43] for review, see Eden and Zeffiro, 1998[44]). However, it has been demonstrated that in nonalphabetic scripts, where reading places less demands on phonemic processing and the integration of visual-orthographic information is crucial, dyslexia is associated with under activity of the left middle frontal gyrus (Siok et al., 2004).[45]
- 1999 Wydell and Butterworth reported the case study of an English-Japanese bilingual with monolingual dyslexia.[46] Suggesting that any language where orthography-to-phonology mapping is transparent, or even opaque, or any language whose orthographic unit representing sound is coarse (i.e. at a whole character or word level) should not produce a high incidence of developmental phonological dyslexia, and that orthograpy can influence dyslexic symptoms
- 2003 A review by Collins and Rourke concluded that the current models of the relation between the brain and dyslexia generally focus on some form of defective or delayed brain maturation.[47]
- 2007 Lyytinen et al. Researchers are seeking a link between the neurological and genetic findings, and the reading disorder.[48]
- 2008 S Heim et al. in a paper "Cognitive subtypes of dyslexia" describe how they compared different sub-groups of dyslexics in comparison with a control group. This is one of the first studies not to just compare dyslexics with a non dyslexic control, but to go further and compared the different cognitive sub groups with a non dyslexic control group.[49]
- Theories of developmental dyslexia
The following theories should not be viewed as competing, but viewed as theories trying to explain the underlying causes of a similar set of symptoms from a variety of research perspectives and background.[original research?]
- Evolutionary hypothesis
This theory posits that reading is an unnatural act, and carried out by humans for an exceedingly brief period in our evolutionary history (Dalby, 1986). It has been less than a hundred years that most western societies promoted reading by the mass population and therefore the forces that shape our behavior have been weak. Many areas of the world still do not have access to reading for the majority of the population. There is no evidence that "pathology" underlies dyslexia but much evidence for cerebral variation or differences. It is these essential differences that are taxed with the artificial task of reading.[50]
- Phonological deficit theory
The phonological deficit theory postulates that people with dyslexia have a specific impairment in the representation, storage and/or retrieval of speech sounds. It explains the reading impairment of people with dyslexia on the basis that learning to read an alphabetic system requires learning the grapheme/phoneme correspondence, i.e. the correspondence between letters and constituent sounds of speech.[51]
- Rapid auditory processing theory
The rapid auditory processing theory is an alternative to the phonological deficit theory, which specifies that the primary deficit lies in the perception of short or rapidly varying sounds. Support for this theory arises from evidence that people with dyslexia show poor performance on a number of auditory tasks, including frequency discrimination and temporal order judgment.[51]
- Visual theory
The visual theory reflects another long standing tradition in the study of dyslexia, that of considering it as a visual impairment giving rise to difficulties with the processing of letters and words on a page of text. This may take the form of unstable binocular fixations, poor vergence, or increased visual crowding. The visual theory does not exclude a phonological deficit.[51]
- Cerebellar theory
Another view is represented by the automaticity/cerebellar theory of dyslexia. Here the biological claim is that the cerebellum of people with dyslexia is mildly dysfunctional and that a number of cognitive difficulties ensue.[51]
- Magnocellular theory
There is a unifying theory that attempts to integrate all the findings mentioned above. A generalization of the visual theory, the magnocellular theory postulates that the magnocellular dysfunction is not restricted to the visual pathways but is generalized to all modalities (visual and auditory as well as tactile).[51]
- Perceptual visual-noise exclusion hypothesis
The concept of a perceptual noise exclusion (Visual-Noise) deficit is an emerging hypothesis, supported by research showing that subjects with dyslexia experience difficulty in performing visual tasks such as motion detection in the presence of perceptual distractions, but do not show the same impairment when the distracting factors are removed in an experimental setting.[52] The researchers have analogized their findings concerning visual discrimination tasks to findings in other research related to auditory discrimination tasks. They assert that dyslexic symptoms arise because of an impaired ability to filter out both visual and auditory distractions, and to categorize information so as to distinguish the important sensory data from the irrelevant.[53]
- Research using brain scan technologies
Modern neuroimaging techniques such as functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) and Positron Emission Tomography (PET) have produced clear evidence of structural differences in the brains of children with reading difficulties. It has been found that people with dyslexia have a deficit in parts of the left hemisphere of the brain involved in reading, which includes the inferior frontal gyrus, inferior parietal lobule, and middle and ventral temporal cortex.[54]
Brain activation studies using PET to study language have produced a breakthrough in our understanding of the neural basis of language over the past decade. A neural basis for the visual lexicon and for auditory verbal short term memory components have been proposed.[55] with some implication that the observed neural manifestation of developmental dyslexia is task-specific (i.e., functional rather than structural)[56]
A University of Hong Kong study argues that dyslexia affects different structural parts of children's brains depending on the language which the children read.[57] The study focused on comparing children that were raised reading English and children raised reading Chinese.
A University of Maastricht (Netherlands) study revealed that adult dyslexic readers underactivate superior temporal cortex for the integration of letters and speech sounds.[58]
- Genetic research
Molecular studies have linked several forms of dyslexia to genetic markers for dyslexia.[59] Several candidate genes have been identified, including at the two regions first related to dyslexia: DCDC2[60] and KIAA0319[61] on chromosome 6,[62] and DYX1C1 on chromosome 15.
A 2007 review reported that no specific cognitive processes are known to be influenced by the proposed susceptibility genes.[63]
A unifying theoretical framework of three working memory components provides a systems perspective for discussing past and new findings in a 12-year research program that point to heterogeneity in the genetic and brain basis and behavioral expression of dyslexia.[64]
- Effect of language orthography
Using both PET and fMRI, Paulescu et al. 2001, show that dyslexia in alphabet writing systems has a universal basis in the brain and can be characterized by the same neurocognitive deficit. Clearly, the manifestation in reading behavior is less severe in a shallow orthography.[43]
- Controversy
Distinguishing between categories of ‘dyslexia’ and ‘poor reader’ or ‘readingdisabled’ are scientifically unsupportable, arbitrary and therefore potentially discriminatory. While the potential of genetics and neuroscience for guiding assessment and educational practice at some stage in the future, there is a mistaken belief that current knowledge in these fields is sufficient to justify a category of dyslexia as a subset of those who encounter reading difficulties.[10]
Management
There is no cure for dyslexia, but dyslexic individuals can learn to read and write with appropriate educational support.
For alphabet writing systems, the fundamental aim is increase a child's awareness of correspondences between graphemes and phonemes, and to relate these to reading and spelling. It has been found that training focused towards visual language and orthographic issues, yields longer-lasting gains than mere oral phonological training.[48]
The best approach is determined by the underlying neurological cause(s) of the dyslexic symptom.
Legal and educational support issues
There are many different national legal statutes and different national special education support structures with regard to special education provision which relate to the management of dyslexia.
See also
- Learning theory (education)
- Irlen filters
- Dysorthographia
- List of people diagnosed with dyslexia
- Net Educational Systems
- Child development
- Dyslexia in fiction
Further Reading
Davis, Ronald (1997). The Gift of Dyslexia: Why Some of the Brightest People Can't Read and How They Can Learn. Souvenir Press Ltd. p. 272. ISBN 0285634127.
Rowan, James (2008). A Memory Therapy & Study Guide For Dyslexia, Adhd, Learning Impairment & Poor Verbal Memory: Improve Your Verbal Memory. Createspace Ltd. ISBN 1440495238.
Neanon, Chris (2002). How to Identify and Support Children with Dyslexia. LDA. p. 64. ISBN 1855033569.
References
- ^ Stanovich, KE. (1988) Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21(10):590-604.
- ^
"Developmental dyslexia in adults: a research review". National Research and Development Centre for Adult Literacy and Numeracy. 2004-05-01. pp. *133-147. Retrieved 2009-05-13.
{{cite news}}
:|first=
missing|last=
(help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b
Castles, A (1993-05). "Varieties of developmental dyslexia". Cognition. 47 (2): 149–80. Retrieved 2009-05-19.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ a b c Habib, M (2000-12). "The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia: an overview and working hypothesis". Brain. 123 (12): 2373–99. PMID 11099442. Retrieved 2009-05-23.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help) - ^ "A Conversation with Sally Shaywitz, M.D., author of Overcoming Dyslexia". Retrieved 2008-04-21.
- ^ Dyslexia: What's the Problem? Medicine Magazine 2008.
- ^ Fletcher, Jack M (1992). "The validity of discrepancy-based definitions of reading disabilities". J Learn Disabil 25(9):555-61, 573. 25 (9): 555–61, 573. ISSN ISSN-0022-2194. Retrieved 2007-06-15.
{{cite journal}}
: Check|issn=
value (help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ a b
Leppänen, P (2002). "Brain responses to changes in speech sound durations differ between infants with and without familial risk for dyslexia". Developmental Neuropsychology. 22 (1): 407–22. PMID 12405511. Retrieved 2009-05-21.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ a b
Richardson, U (2003). "Speech perception of infants with high familial risk for dyslexia differ at the age of 6 months". Developmental Neuropsychology. 23 (3): 385–97. PMID 12740192. Retrieved 2009-05-21.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ a b ELLIOTT, JULIAN G. (2008). "Does Dyslexia Exist?". Journal of Philosophy of Education,. 42 (3–4): 475–491. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9752.2008.00653.x. Retrieved 2009-06-22.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help)CS1 maint: extra punctuation (link) - ^ a b Katz, Jack (2007-05-14). "APD Evaluation to Therapy: The Buffalo Model". AudiologyOnline. Retrieved 2009-05-16.
- ^ Ramus, Franck (2003-05). "Developmental dyslexia: specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction?". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 13 (2): 212–218. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00035-7. Retrieved 2009-05-19.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help) - ^ Moncrieff, Deborah (2004-02-02). "Temporal Processing Deficits in Children with Dyslexia". speechpathology.com. speechpathology.com. Retrieved 2009-05-13.
{{cite news}}
: Check|authorlink=
value (help); External link in
(help)|authorlink=
- ^ Moncrieff, Deborah (2002-09-23). "Auditory Processing Disorders and Dyslexic Children". audiologyonline.com. audiologyonline.com. Retrieved 2009-05-13.
{{cite news}}
: Check|authorlink=
value (help); External link in
(help)|authorlink=
- ^
Kruk, Richard (2007-08-31). "Visual processing characteristics of children with Meares–Irlen syndrome". Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics. 28 (1, ): 35–46. doi:10.1111/j.1475-1313.2007.00532.x. Retrieved 2009-05-16.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help)CS1 maint: extra punctuation (link) - ^ Evans, Bruce (1995-04-07). "Optometric correlates of Meares–Irlen Syndrome: a matched group study". Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics. 15 (5): 481–487. doi:10.1046/j.1475-1313.1995.9500063j.x. Retrieved 2009-05-16.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Ramus, Franck (2002-07-25). "The relationship between motor control and phonology in dyslexic children" (PDF). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 44 (5): 712–722. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00157. Retrieved 2009-05-19.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^
Rochelle, Kim (2008-10-02). "Symptoms of hyperactivity and inattention can mediate deficits of postural stability in developmental dyslexia". Exp Brain Res. Springer-Verlag. pp. 627–633. doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1568-5. Retrieved 2009-05-14.
{{cite news}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^
Gerritsa, Ellen (2009-05). "Early language development of children at familial risk of dyslexia: Speech perception and production". Journal of Communication Disorders Volume 42, Issue 3, May-June 2009, Pages 180-194. 42 (3): 180–194. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2008.10.004. Retrieved 2009-05-24.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help)CS1 maint: extra punctuation (link) - ^ Cluttering as a Complex of Learning Disabilities
- ^ Miles, T.R. (1983). Dyslexia: the Pattern of Difficulties. Oxford: Blackwell.
- ^ Stephen Wilcox - Dyslexia & Vision
- ^ This book is written in "plain language" to make it easier to read for student with dyslexia as well as "busy" teachers and
- ^ BERKHAN O. Neur. Zent 28 1917
- ^ Wagner, Rudolph (January, 1973). "Rudolf Berlin: Originator of the term dyslexia". Annals of Dyslexia. 23 (Number 1): 57–63. doi:10.1007/BF02653841. Retrieved 2009-05-12.
{{cite journal}}
:|issue=
has extra text (help); Check date values in:|date=
(help) - ^ "Uber Dyslexie". Archiv fur Psychiatrie. 15: 276–278.
- ^ Snowling, Margaret J. (1996-11-02). "Dyslexia: a hundred years on". BMJ. 313 (7065): 1096. PMID 8916687. Retrieved 2007-06-08.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help) - ^ Hinshelwood, J. (1917). Congenital Word-blindness. HK Lewis \& Co., ltd.
- ^ Orton, Samuel (1925). "Word-blindness in school children". Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry. 14 (5): 285–516. Retrieved 2009-05-19.
- ^ Henry, MK (1998). "Structured, sequential, multisensory teaching: The Perlow legacy". Annals of Dyslexia. 48: 1. doi:10.1007/s11881-998-0002-9.
- ^ Orton, S.T. (1928). "Specific reading disability—strephosymbolia". Journal of the American Medical Association. 90 (14): 1095–1099.
- ^ Goeke, Jennifer (2006). "Orton-Gillingham and Orton-Gillingham-based reading instruction: a review of the literature". Journal of Special Education.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Bradley, L (1983). "Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A Causal connection". Nature. 30 (2): 419–421. doi:10.1038/301419a0.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Galaburda, Albert M. (1979-02-27). "Cytoarchitectonic Abnormalities in Developmental Dyslexia: A Case Study". Annals of Neurology. 6 (2): 94–100. doi:10.1002/ana.410060203. Retrieved 2009-06-02.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Galaburda, A M (1985-08). ".Developmental Dyslexia: four consecutive patients with cortical anomalies". Annals Neurology. 18 (2): 222–33. PMID 4037763. Retrieved 2009-05-23.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^
Cohen, M. (1989-06). "Neuropathological abnormalities in developmental dysphasia". Annals Neurology. 25 (6): 567–70. PMID 2472772. Retrieved 2009-05-23.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^
Manis, F R (1996-02). "On the bases of two subtypes of developmental [corrected] dyslexia". Cognition. 58 (2): 157–95. PMID 8820386. Retrieved 2009-05-25.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); line feed character in|title=
at position 47 (help) - ^
Galaburda, A.M. (1994-08-16). "Evidence for Aberrant Auditory Anatomy in Developmental Dyslexia". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 91 (17): 8010–8013. doi:10.1073/pnas.91.17.8010. PMID 8058748. Retrieved 2007-06-17.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^
Fiez, J A (1998-02-03). "Neuroimaging studies of word reading". Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 95 (3): 914–21. PMID 9448259. Retrieved 2009-05-27.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^
Turkeltaub, P E (2002-07). "Meta-analysis of the functional neuroanatomy of single-word reading: method and validation". Neuroimage. 16 (3 pt1): 765–80. PMID 12169260. Retrieved 2009-05-27.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^
Gelfand, J R (2003-06-05). "Dissociating neural mechanisms of temporal sequencing and processing phonemes". Neuron. 38 (5): 831–42. PMID 12797966. Retrieved 2009-05-27.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^
Poldrack, R A (1999-07). "Functional specialization for semantic and phonological processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex". Neuroimage. 10 (1): 15–35. PMID 10385578. Retrieved 2009-05-27.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ a b
Paulesu, E (2001-03-16). "Dyslexia: Cultural Diversity and Biological Unity". Science. 291 (551): 2165–2167. doi:10.1126/science.1057179. Retrieved 2009-05-23.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ {{cite journal title=Neural Systems Affected in Developmental Dyslexia Revealed by Functional Neuroimaging |journal=Neuron |date= 1998-08 |first=Guinevere F. |last=Eden |coauthors= Thomas A. Zeffiro |volume=21 |issue=2 |pages=279-282 |id= doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80537-1 |url=http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0896627300805371 |accessdate=2009-05-23 }}
- ^
Eden, Guinevere (2004-10-28). "Neural Changes following Remediation in Adult Developmental Dyslexia (Clinical Study)". Neuron. 44 (3): 411–422. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.019. Retrieved 2009-05-20.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^
Wydell, Taeko Nakayama (1999-04-01). "A case study of an English-Japanese bilingual with monolingual dyslexia". Cognition. 70 (3): 273–305. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00016-5. Retrieved 2009-05-26.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Collins, David W. (2003-10-07). "Learning-disabled Brains: A Review of the Literature". Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 25 (7): 1011. PMID 1034 doi:10.1076/jcen.25.7.1011.16487. Retrieved 2009-06-17.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ a b Lyytinen, Heikki, Erskine, Jane, Aro, Mikko, Richardson, Ulla (2007), "Reading and reading disorders", in Hoff, Erika (ed.), Blackwell Handbook of Language Development, Blackwell, pp. 454–474, ISBN 9781405132534
{{citation}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Heim, S (2008). "Cognitive subtypes of dyslexia". Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars). 68 (1): 73–82. PMID 18389017. Retrieved 2009-06-02.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Dalby, J.T. (1986). "An ultimate view of reading ability". International Journal of Neuroscience. 30 (3). Gordon and Breach, Science Publishers, Inc.: 227–230. PMID 3759349.
- ^ a b c d e Ramus, Franck (2003). "Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults". Brain. 126 (4). Oxford University Press: 841–865. doi:10.1093/brain/awg076. PMID 12615643. Retrieved 2007-05-27.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ Sperling, Anne J. (2006). "Motion-Perception Deficits and Reading Impairment: It's the Noise, Not the Motion". Psychological Science. 17 (12). Association for Psychological Science: 1047–1053. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01825.x.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^
Sperling, Anne J. (2005). "Deficits in perceptual noise exclusion in developmental dyslexia". Nature Neuroscience. 8: 862–863. doi:10.1038/nn1474. ISSN 1097-6256. Retrieved 2007-06-12.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Fan Cao, Tali Bitan, Tai-Li Chou, Douglas D. Burman, James R. Booth (2006). "Deficient orthographic and phonological representations in children with dyslexia revealed by brain activation patterns" (PDF). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry (November 2006). 47: 1041–1050. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01684.x. Retrieved 2007-10-06.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^
Chertkow, H (1997). "PET activation and language". Clinical Neuroscience NY. 4 (2): 78–86. Retrieved 2009-05-19.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^
McCrory, E (2000-09). "Abnormal functional activation during a simple word repetition task: A PET study of adult dyslexics". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 12 (5): 753-62. PMID 11054918. Retrieved 2009-05-19.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Wai Ting Siok (8 April 2008). "A structural–functional basis for dyslexia in the [[cortex]] of Chinese readers". The National Academy of Sciences of the USA. Retrieved 2009-05-05.
{{cite web}}
: Cite has empty unknown parameter:|coauthors=
(help); URL–wikilink conflict (help) - ^ Blau, V (2009-03-12). "Reduced neural integration of letters and speech sounds links phonological and reading deficits in adult dyslexia". Current Biology. 19 (6): 503–8. PMID 1928540. Retrieved 2009-05-23.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Grigorenko, EL (1997). "Susceptibility loci for distinct components of developmental dyslexia on chromosomes 6 and 15". American journal of human genetics. 60 (1): 27–39.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ Meng, H (2005). "DCDC2 is associated with reading disability and modulates neuronal development in the brain". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 102 (47): 17053–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.0508591102. PMID 16278297.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ Paracchini, S (2008-12). "Association of the KIAA0319 dyslexia susceptibility gene with reading skills in the general population". American Journal of Psychiatry. 165 (12): 1576–84. PMID 18829873. Retrieved 2009-05-26.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help); Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Grigorenko, EL (2000). "Chromosome 6p influences on different dyslexia-related cognitive processes: further confirmation". American journal of human genetics. 66 (2): 715–23. doi:10.1086/302755.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^
Schumacher, Johannes (16 February 2007). "Genetics of dyslexia: the evolving landscape". Journal of Medical Genetics. 44. BMJ Publishing Group: 289–297. doi:10.1136/jmg.2006.046516. PMID 17307837. Retrieved 2007-05-27.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^
Berninger, VW (2008). "A multidisciplinary approach to understanding developmental dyslexia within working-memory architecture: genotypes, phenotypes, brain, and instruction". Dev Neuropsychol. 2008;33(6):707-44. 33 (6): 707–44. Retrieved 2009-05-18.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help)
External links
- Research papers, articles and media
- Auditory and visual automatic attention deficits in developmental dyslexia
- Auditory and Visual Dynamic Processing: Separate Influences in Reading?
- Auditory frequency discrimination in adult developmental dyslexics
- Can sensitivity to auditory frequency modulation predict children's phonological and reading skills?
- Dynamic sensory sensitivity and children’s word decoding skills
- Effect of intensive training on auditory processing and reading skills
- On the Relationship between Dynamic Visual and Auditory Processing and Literacy Skills; Results from a Large Primary-school Study
- Haskins Laboratories reading research
- Professor Dorothy Bishop: Evaluating Alternative Solutions for Dyslexia